Sunteți pe pagina 1din 14

Joint Attention and Conversation in Mother-Infant-Sibling Triads Author(s): Michelle E.

Barton and Michael Tomasello Source: Child Development, Vol. 62, No. 3 (Jun., 1991), pp. 517-529 Published by: Blackwell Publishing on behalf of the Society for Research in Child Development Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1131127 Accessed: 24/04/2009 15:42
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=black. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit organization founded in 1995 to build trusted digital archives for scholarship. We work with the scholarly community to preserve their work and the materials they rely upon, and to build a common research platform that promotes the discovery and use of these resources. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Blackwell Publishing and Society for Research in Child Development are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Child Development.

http://www.jstor.org

Joint Attention and Conversation Triads Mother-Infant-Sibling

in

Michelle E. Barton and Michael Tomasello


Emory University
MICHELLE andTOMASELLO, MICHAEL. BARTON, E., Joint Attention and Conversation in MotherDEVELOPMENT, 62, 517-529. The current study investigated 1991, Infant-Sibling Triads. CHILD the general nature of joint attentional and conversational interaction in mother-infant-sibling triads. 9 19-month-oldinfants and 9 24-month-oldinfants were videotaped during 20 min of free play with their mothers and preschool-aged siblings arounda common activity. Analyses revealed that even 19-month-oldinfants were capable of participatingin triadic interactionsand conversations, and that the proportionalfrequency of both these measures increased with age. Triadic conversations were nearly 3 times longer and elicited nearly twice as many infant turns per conversation as dyadic conversations. Infants were more likely to join into an ongoing conversational topic than to initiate one themselves, and they were more likely to take a turn in those conversations if they were in a joint attentional state with the speaker. Infants were just as likely to respond to a comment or request directed to another person as they were to one directed to themselves, indicating reliable comprehension of language not addressed to them. These results suggest that the mother-infant-sibling interactive context differs in important ways from the mother-infant dyadic context and that it is a richer language learning environment than previously supposed.

Most research examining the effects of social interaction on language development has focused on Western, middle-class mother-infant dyads. Converging lines of evidence from this literature have led to the belief that frequent dyadic interactions with a responsive, nondirective adult are conducive to early language learning (e.g., Della Corte, Benedict, & Klein, 1983; Nelson, 1973; Olson, Bayles, & Bates, 1986; Rice, 1989; Tomasello & Farrar, 1986). Dyadic interaction with adults is clearly not necessary for language acquisition, however, as many children in non-Western cultures acquire language with few, if any, such interactions. Many of these children are immersed from birth almost exclusively in multispeaker contexts, especially those involving multiple children (Ochs, 1982; Schieffelin, 1979; Schieffelin & Ochs, 1983). This pattern is typical of many later-born children in Western culture as well, and it is becoming more prevalent for firstborn children, who spend more and more of their time with peers in day-care or nursery school settings. The multichild context is clearly an important one in the language acquisition of most of the world's children.

The few existing studies of multichild contexts have focused almost exclusively on how the presence of another child affects adult-child dyadic interactions. Several studies have reported that the presence of another child reduces both the overall quantity and the overall quality of adult-child interactions. linguistic and nonlinguistic Adults in multichild contexts directly address each child with fewer utterances (Jones & Adamson, 1987; Woollett, 1986), and they become more directive in their interactive styles (Schaffer & Liddell, 1984; Tomasello, Mannle, & Kruger, 1986)-both well-known negative correlates of early language growth (e.g., Tomasello & Farrar, 1986; Wells, 1981). These "negative" influences on the adult in multichild contexts have thus been posited as a possible explanation for the slower rate of vocabulary growth found in twins (e.g., Tomasello et al., 1986) and, to a lesser extent, in later-born children (Jones & Adamson, 1987; Nelson, 1973; Nelson, Baker, Denninger, Bonvillian, & Kaplan, 1985). The clear implication is that the multichild context is a less than optimum language learning environment because the language learning children have

The authors would like to thank the mothers and children who made this study possible. Thanks also to Ann Krugerand Amy Ledeberg for helpful comments on a previous version of the manuscript.Portions of this researchwere presented at the Conference on Human Development, Richmond, VA, March 1990. Requests for reprints may be addressed to either author at the Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta,GA 30322.
[Child Development, 1991, 62, 517-529. ? 1991 by the Society for Researchin Child Development, Inc. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/91/6203-0010$01.00]

518

Child Development
guistic skills in order to be accepted into the ongoing conversation. The current study attempted to look more deeply into the dynamics of motherinfant-sibling triadic interactions. We focused on triads containing either a 19- or 24-month-old infant, along with their preschool-age sibling. Our first aim was to document the nonlinguistic joint attentional interactions that took place in these triads. Joint attention has been shown to provide important nonlinguistic scaffolding for the linguistic interactions of 1-2-year-old children in dyadic contexts (see Tomasello, 1988, for a review), and so it was expected that it might be important in triadic contexts as well. Our second aim was to establish some basic quantitative parameters of the conversations that took place in motherinfant-sibling triadic contexts, that is, basic quantitative information on the relative frequency of mother-infant, mother-sibling, and mother-infant-sibling infant-sibling, conversations, and the average length of each of these. We hypothesized that conversations involving all three participants would occur at both ages but would be more frequent and longer for the 24-month-olds. Our final aim was to determine more specifically the ways in which infants under 2 years of age, whose linguistic and conversational skills are considerably more modest than those of the children studied by Dunn and Shatz, participate in conversations in the triadic context. We sought to determine how often they initiated, joined into, and continued conversations in this context, as well as some of the linguistic and nonlinguistic factors that might facilitate their participation.

to "share access" to caregivers with their co-twins, siblings, or peers. Recently, however, it has been suggested that there may be positive effects of participating in multichild (especially sibling) interactive contexts. The posited benefits in all cases involve pragmatic skills rather than the more strictly linguistic skills, such as vocabulary size, focused on by previous investigators. For example, Woollett (1986) argued that while homes with older siblings may lessen adult sensitivity to individual children, they also provide each child with a more stimulating linguistic environment with a variety of salient language models and communicative styles (see also Schaffer, 1989). Similarly, Mannle and Tomasello (1987) argued that communicating with siblings forces young children to adapt their linguistic skills for speakers who are less able (and perhaps less willing) than primary caregivers to take their perspective or assent to their wishes. Also, the multispeaker context may allow the language learning child to "overhear" language among other people, which may provide linguistic models helpful in such areas as the acquisition of deictic terms (e.g., see Oshima-Takane, 1988) and third-party reference (see Forrester, 1988). One very obvious benefit of multispeaker contexts for pragmatic skills is the opportunity they provide for participation in multispeaker conversations. The home setting with siblings is an obvious first step in this direction, but, surprisingly, almost no research has been conducted on parent conversations with multiple siblings. The only study to focus on such conversations was by Dunn and Shatz (1989), who investigated toddlers' abilities to join into ongoing conversations. mother-sibling They recorded the behavior of 2-3-year-olds in a home context with both mother and a preschool-age sibling present, focusing on the younger siblings' utterances that were "intrusions" into the conversations of their mothers and older siblings. They found that the younger siblings were quite capable of understanding conversations not directly involving them, as evidenced by their increasing ability to join in. Moreover, the proportion of toddler statements contributing new information to the conversation was actually higher when they were intruding than when they were responding to speech addressed directly to them. This presumably indicates that the joining in process encouraged the children to use their most sophisticated lin-

Method
Subjects
Subjects were recruited from a psychology department subjects file composed of parents who responded voluntarily to a letter (mailed to parents of newborns) soliciting cooperation for studies in child development. Mothers were contacted by phone and asked about their infants' language progress; only infants who were regularly producing language were invited to participate. Two groups ofmothet-infant-sibling triads served as subjects. Thk first group of subjects included nine infants (four males and five females) between the ages of 19 and 20 months (M = 19.2 months, SD = .4 months; mean MLU 1.28 words), their preschoolaged siblings between the ages of 3 and 5 years (M = 4.4 years, SD = .5 years), and

Barton and Tomasello


their mothers. The second group of subjects included nine infants (five males and four females) between the ages of 23 and 25 months (M = 24.2 months, SD = .4 months; mean MLU 1.70 words), their preschoolaged siblings between the ages of 3 and 5 years (M = 4.3 years, SD = .5 years), and their mothers. (Two additional triads, one in each group, were dropped from the study when they produced almost no language in the observation session.) Observational Procedure Each mother-infant-sibling triad was videotaped for 20 min of free play in the psychology department's child observation playroom. The play situation was centered on tactile exploration activities using a bin (86 x 71 x 10 cm) of uncooked rice and age-appropriate sand toys such as cups, scoops, trucks, and sieves. A cameraperson and the experimenter were also present in the room but remained as unobtrusive as possible. Mothers were instructed to play freely with their children as they typically would at home, except that on four occasions (at 5-min intervals) the experimenter gave the mother or the sibling a new toy to introduce to one of the two others. This procedure was meant to simulate four types of naturally occurring dyadic exchanges: mother-infant, sibling-infant, sibling-mother, and mothersibling. This ensured that, for each infant, both the mother and the sibling directly initiated interaction with the infant at least once, and that there were at least two opportunities for the infant to overhear conversations between the mother and sibling. Coding Procedure Each videotape was coded in a number of ways, falling into three general categories: joint attention, all conversations, and infantinvolved conversations. the videotapes Joint attention.-First, were coded for joint attentional episodes (JAEs) using a procedure based on that of Tomasello and Todd (1983). JAEs were defined as social interactions between two or three participants who shared the same attentional focus (usually an object) at the same time for at least 3 sec (brief diversions to a nonsocial activity within a shared focus were ignored). To qualify as a social interaction, at least one participant had to acknowledge during the joint focus the participation of the other(s), either by establishing eye contact or by making an appropriate verbal or nonverbal response.

519

The first author and a trained research assistant coded the videotapes continuously for JAE type: mother-infant, sibling-infant, mother-sibling, and triadic. (Sibling-infant interactions, however, never occurred and hence were not analyzed, nor were portions of the tapes where no interaction occurred.) For purposes of reliability analyses, 16 5-min segments were randomly chosen from the videotapes, with the restriction that no two segments came from the same triad; these were coded independently by the two coders. Agreement was calculated on a second-by-second basis, where agreement meant that both coders identified the same type of JAE for a given second (Bakeman & Gottman, 1986). Percent agreement was .85 and Cohen's kappa was .77, both well within acceptable ranges. For each JAE type, three quantitative measures were computed for each triad: (1) proportional frequency, computed as the number of a given type as a proportion of total JAEs; (2) proportional time, computed as the time spent in a given type as a proportion of total time in JAEs; and (3) average time, computed as the number of seconds in a given type divided by the number of JAEs of that type. All conversations.-Two pairs of research assistants made written transcriptions of all language occurring during each session, along with notes on nonverbal exchanges such as nods, gestures, and vocalizations. These were checked and corrected where necessary by the first author. All conversational measures were coded from these transcripts by the first author. The second coding was aimed at assessing the overall conversational behavior of the triads. Conversations were defined as a series of exchanges between two to three speakers (at least one turn each) that shared the same topic. Topic was defined as what was being talked about-this could be as specific as "the funnel" or as general as "the eating game"-and coders wrote down a brief verbal description of the topic of each conversation. Conversation type was determined by the participants and thus included the same four possibilities as JAE type: mother-infant, sibling-infant, mothersibling, and triadic. (Sibling-infant conversations were so rare, occurring less than 2% of the time, that they were not analyzed, nor was language that was not a part of any conversation.)

520

Child Development
For each infant turn in the first three of these turn type categories (i.e., all infant first turns), it was determined whether or not that turn was successful at continuing or starting a conversation, that is, whether or not the subsequent speaker took a turn on the infant's topic. Thus the ratio of success in each of these categories was the number of infant turns of a given turn type that were followed by an on-topic response by mother or sibling, divided by the total number of infant turns of that same type. Both the turn type and success classifications required purely mechanical tallying from the transcripts that had already been coded for general conversational topic, and thus further measures of reliability beyond the original coding into topics were considered unnecessary. Singled out for further analysis were those infant-involved dyadic and triadic conversations where the infant joined into an ongoing conversational topic. Of particular interest were differences between the utterances immediately preceding an infant turn (either the Join Topic turn or the subsequent Continue Topic turns) and other utterances that were not followed by an infant turn. With this in mind, each mother and sibling utterance within these conversations was coded as to: (1) addressee (infant-directed and other-directed, based on who was being addressed); (2) speech act (question, comment, or request); and (3) joint attention (whether or not the infant was in a joint attentional state with the speaker). Combining these codes yielded a total of 12 different utterance types to which the infant might or might not respond: infant-directed questions, comments, or requests inside JAEs; infant-directed questions, comments, or requests outside JAEs; other-directed questions, comments, or requests inside JAEs; other-directed questions, comments, or requests outside JAEs. For each infant, the probability of responding to each utterance type was calculated as the number of times the infant verbally responded on topic to a particular utterance type divided by the total number of that type (i.e., number of responses as a function of number of opportunities to respond). for these three coding Reliability schemes was determined as follows. Addressee was coded by the transcribers as they transcribed from videotape. Four randomly chosen videotapes (20%) were then independently transcribed and coded for ad-

For purposes of reliability, 16 5-min segments were randomly chosen from the transcripts, with the restriction that no two segments came from the same triad. These segments were coded independently by a trained research assistant. Agreement with the original coder was calculated on an basis, where both utterance-by-utterance coders had to agree on both type and topic for a given utterance for that observation to be considered as an agreement. Judgments of topic were considered in agreement if both coders mentioned the same object or same general activity (e.g., "making rice piles" was considered the same as "making rice castles"). Percent agreement was .86. (This coding procedure was not amenable to Cohen's kappa, because the topic code did not consist of a finite set of categories that could be specified prior to coding.) For each conversation type, three quantitative measures were computed for each triad: (1) proportional frequency, computed as the number of conversations of a given type as a proportion of all conversations; (2) average length, computed as the average number of turns per conversation for a given type (a turn was defined as one speaker's utterance[s] bounded by another speaker's turn or at least 5 sec of silence; a turn could be any vocalization, unless otherwise noted); and (3) average number of participant turns, computed as the average number of each person's turns per conversation for a given type. conversations.-The Infant-involved third and final set of measures was aimed at assessing the conditions under which infants participated in dyadic and triadic conversations with their mothers and siblings. We began by identifying infant utterances and determining how they were used in terms of topic maintenance. There were four types of infant turn. (1) If the infant produced an utterance that was the first turn on a topic, and that turn was preceded by more than a 5-sec silence, the turn was coded as Initiate Topic. (2) If the infant produced an utterance that was the first turn on a topic, and that turn was not preceded by more than a 5-sec silence (i.e., the others were talking about something else when the infant took her turn), the turn was coded as Change Topic. (3) If the infant made a contribution to a topic already established by the mother or sibling, her first turn was coded as Join Topic. (4) All subsequent infant turns on an established topic were coded as Continue Topic. This variable was called turn type.

Barton and Tomasello


dressee by the first author. Agreement was calculated on an utterance-by-utterance basis, where both coders had to agree on both who was speaking and who was being addressed. Percent agreement was .96. Speech act was coded by the first author. A trained research assistant independently coded a randomly chosen 20% of the target conversations. Percent agreement was .96. The joint attention determination required only a matching of the time at which an utterance occurred with the times in the JAE coding, and thus reliability analyses were deemed unnecessary.

521

Results
Results are presented in three sections: joint attention, all conversations, and infantinvolved conversations. In all cases in which the dependent variable was a proportion, analyses were done both on the original proportions and on the values resulting from an arcsine transformation. Statistical values for the transformed data are reported in the text, whereas the tables and figure present the original, untransformed means and standard deviations. In all cases the analyses on the transformed and untransformed data yielded the same results. Joint Attention During the 20 min of taped interaction, the triads engaged in an average of 36 JAEs when infants were 19 months of age and an average of 35 JAEs when infants were 24 months of age. The proportional frequencies of the JAE types were different at the two

ages, however, as may be seen in Table 1. Because for each subject proportions summed to unity across types, an overall 2 x 3 ANOVA could not be performed on these data. Since our primary interest was in developmental changes, our analytic strategy was to perform a t test on the JAE type showing the least developmental change (mother-infant), and to perform a 2 (age) x 2 (JAE type) mixed ANOVA on the other two types (mother-sibling and mother-siblinginfant). The t test found no significant change in the mean proportion of motherinfant JAEs as a function of child age (approximately 34% at both ages). The 2 (age) x 2 (JAE type) mixed ANOVA performed on the remaining data found a main effect of JAE type. The mean proportion of mother-sibling JAEs (.37) was greater than that of mother-infant-sibling JAEs (.29), F(1,16) = 5.48, p < .05. The significant interaction, F(1,16) = 10.40, p < .01, however, suggests that the main effect of JAE type is due mainly to differences at the younger age only. Tukey's (a) test for unconfounded means (Linton & Gallo, 1975) confirmed this finding: only at 19 months was the mean proportion of mother-sibling JAEs greater than that of mother-infant-sibling JAEs (p < .05). The Tukey test also revealed a significant increase in the mean proportion of mother-infant-sibling JAEs with age (p < .05); the mother-sibling decrease was not a significant one. (The pattern of results was similar when the average time of JAEs was analyzed; however, these comparisons did

TABLE 1 MEAN (and PROPORTIONS StandardDeviations) OFJOINT


ATTENTION AND CONVERSATIONTYPES AS A FUNCTION OF INFANT AGE INFANT AGE

INTERACTION TYPES
Joint attention: Mother-infant .................... Mother-sibling.................... Mother-infant-sibling........... Conversation: Mother-infant .................... Mother-sibling.................... Mother-infant-sibling...........

19 Months
.35 (.10) .42 (.08)a .24 (.08) .23 (.12) .62 (.12)a .15 (.09)

24 Months
.33 (.13) .32 (.11) .35 (.09)b .28 (.07) .43 (.15)e .27 (.16)b

NOTE.-Joint attention proportions are based on a range of 20 to 51 JAEs per triad. Conversation proportions are based on a range of 16 to 38 conversations per triad. "a Mother-sibling greater than mother-infant-sibling, p < .05. b 24 months greater than 19 months, p < .05. c 19 months greater than 24 months, p < .05.

522

Child Development
mother-infant conversations as a function of child age (approximately 25% at both ages). The 2 (age) x 2 (conversation type) mixed ANOVA found a main effect of conversation type such that the mean proportion of mother-sibling conversations (.53) was greater than that of mother-infant-sibling conversations (.21), F(1,16) = 28.75, p < .01. The significant age x type interaction, F(1,16) = 6.67, p < .05, however, suggests that the main effect of conversation type is due to differences at the younger age only. Tukey's (a) test for unconfounded means confirmed this finding (p < .05): only at 19 months was the mean proportion of mothersibling conversations greater than that of mother-infant-sibling conversations. In addition, the decrease in mother-sibling conversations with age and the increase in conversations with mother-infant-sibling age both were significant changes (p < .05). 2 presents the Average length.-Table average length of each conversation type (in turns). The data on which these values are based were subjected to a 2 (age) x 3 (type) mixed ANOVA. While there was no main effect of age, there was a significant main effect of conversation type, F(2,32) = 28.53, p < .01. Means for each type were: motherinfant, 3.79; mother-sibling, 5.07; and mother-infant-sibling, 13.72. A Tukey post hoc analysis found that the average length of mother-infant-sibling conversations was significantly greater than both mother-infant and mother-sibling conversations (p < .01), while the latter two types did not differ significantly. The interaction was not significant. Turns.-Also presented in Table 2 is the average number of turns taken by each participant in each conversation type. In the case of dyadic conversations, the turns of each participant should be highly correlated with the total length of conversations of that same type; this is because turns were defined as alternating, and thus no one participant could take more than 50% of the turns unless they both began and ended conversations. In the case of triadic conversations, however, there are no constraints on the number of turns any one participant could take (outside of the stipulation that each individual take at least one turn). In order to evaluate individuals' contributions to triadic conversations, we therefore analyzed the average number of each participant's turns per conversation type using age x conversation type mixed ANOVAs, one for each partici-

not reach significance. This indicates that triads spent roughly equal average lengths of time in JAEs of all types at both ages.) We were also interested in how triadic JAEs started. In particular, we were interested in whether the majority of triadic JAEs began by the infant joining into an ongoing mother-sibling dyadic interaction, by the sibling joining into an ongoing motherinfant dyadic interaction, or by all three members participating equally from the beginning. To assess these possibilities, the proportional distribution of mother-infant, mother-sibling, and "null" JAEs (i.e., periods of no interaction) immediately preceding each triadic JAE was calculated. Because 23% of the triadic JAEs began at the onset of a 5-min recording session when a new toy was introduced, the preceding joint attentional state was not comparable in these cases (one participant had left the room to get the new toy), and hence these triadic JAEs were excluded from analysis. The 2 (age) x 3 (JAE type) mixed ANOVA performed on the remaining data found a main effect of the JAE type immediately preceding each triadic JAE, F(2,32) = 4.03, p < .05. A Tukey post hoc analysis found that the mean proportion of null episodes preceding the triadic JAEs (.36) was significantly greater than that of mother-infant JAEs (.16), p < .05. The mean proportion of mothersibling JAEs preceding the triadic JAEs (.24) was not significantly different from those of null or mother-infant episodes. It is thus clear that the infant was an active participant in the initiation of the vast majority of triadic JAEs. All Conversations the Proportional frequency.--During 20 min of taped interaction, the triads engaged in an average of 27.3 conversations of one type or another when infants were 19 months of age and an average of 26.7 conversations when infants were 24 months of age. The proportional frequencies of the different conversation types, however, differ as a function of child age, as can be seen in Table 1. Once again, because proportions across type for each subject summed to unity, an overall 2 x 3 ANOVA could not be performed on these data. Thus our analytic strategy was to perform a t test on the conversation type showing the least developmental change (mother-infant) and to perform a 2 (age) x 2 (conversation type) mixed ANOVA on the other two types (mothersibling and mother-infant-sibling). The t test found no change in the mean proportion of

Barton and Tomasello


TABLE 2

523

MEAN NUMBER OF PARTICIPANTTURNS (and Standard Deviations) AS A FUNCTION OF CONVERSATIONTYPE CONVERSATIONTYPES

PARTICIPANT TURNS Mother....................... .............................

Mother-Infant 2.11
...

Mother-Sibling 2.52
... 2.54

Mother-InfantSibling 6.70
3.01 4.00 (3.38)a (2.00)b (2.06)b (6.66)a

(.79)
(.74)

(.79)
...

1.85 Infant ........................... .................... Sibling .................................. .. ........... ..

(.83)

Total average length (in turns)........... 3.79

(1.13)

5.07

(1.56)

13.71

NOTE.-The number of conversations on which the means are based range from 2 to 12 for mother-infant, 7 to 29 for mother-sibling, and 1 to 17 for mother-infant-sibling. "a Mother-infant-sibling greater than mother-infant and mother-sibling, p < .01. b Mother-infant-sibling greater than mother-infant or mother-sibling, p < .05.

pant. (These were 2 x 2 ANOVAs for infants and siblings since they only participated in two conversation types; because mothers participated in all three conversation types, their data were analyzed in a 2 x 3 ANOVA.) There was no main effect of age on the average number of participant turns in any of the analyses. In all three analyses, however, there was a main effect of conversation type, such that each participant took more turns in triadic than in dyadic conversations: infant, F(1,16) = 4.95, p < .05; sibling, F(1,16) = 8.29, p < .05; mother, F(2,32) = 23.79, p < .01. (Tukey post hoc analysis revealed that the mothers took more turns in triadic conversations than in mother-infant or mothersibling conversations, the latter two not being significantly different.) None of the interactions was significant. sations.-The findings that triadic conversations were nearly three times as long as dyadic conversations, with each participant taking many more turns, were so striking that two further possibilities were considered. First, it is possible that the motherinfant-sibling conversations were not truly triadic because one participant (especially the infant) may not have participated equally with the others. The first thing to note in this regard is that the infants took nearly as many turns per triadic conversation (3.01) as did the siblings (4.00) (see Table 2). In addition, however, we examined the triadic conversations in more detail in order to analyze each participant's relative contribution. Three subtypes of triadic conversation emerged. The first type was characterized by full reciprocation in which each participant both addressed and was addressed by each of the other participants; these comprised 28% of the triadic conversations. The second type

Further analysis of triadic conver-

was characterized by partial reciprocation in which each participant either addressed or was addressed by each other participant; these comprised 50% of the triadic conversations. In roughly half of these partially reciprocating conversations, the infant both addressed and was addressed by each of the other participants. The last type was characterized by no reciprocation between two of the participants (i.e., they never addressed each other); these comprised the remaining 23% of the triadic conversations. In almost all cases the "missing link" in these conversations was between the infant and sibling. Thus, in terms of the infant's participation overall, in 78% of the triadic conversations (all of the fully and partially reciprocating ones) the infant either addressed or was addressed by each of the other participants; in 53% of the triadic conversations (all of the fully reciprocating and half of the partially reciprocating ones) the infant both addressed and was addressed by each of the other participants. The second possibility is that the infants' dyadic conversations with their mothers were shorter than triadic conversations, with fewer infant turns, because they were interrupted by siblings attempting to begin a new topic. There is some a priori validity to this hypothesis, as on average 48% of the mother-infant conversations ended because they were interrupted by siblings (of the remaining mother-infant conversations, 24% ended naturally with a pause of silence and 30% ended when mother or infant decided to change topic). However, comparison of interrupted and uninterrupted mother-infant conversations showed them to be of nearly identical length, 3.5 turns for interrupted (SD = 2.7 turns) and 3.4 turns for uninterrupted (SD = 2.2 turns). Thus, when the av-

524

Child Development
ferent (p < .05). The interaction was not significant. Success.-An overall 2 (age) x 3 (turn type) mixed ANOVA was not performed on the success data because of low frequencies of Initiate Topic: four of the younger infants and two of the older infants did not initiate any new topics. The mean success rates for those infants who did attempt to initiate conversations were .77 for the five younger infants and .69 for the seven older infants. Initiations were thus omitted from further analysis, and a 2 (age) x 2 (turn type) mixed ANOVA was performed on the success ratios of the other two turn types (Join Topic, Change Topic). This analysis found no main effect of age. There was a significant main effect of turn type: the success rate for joining a conversation with an already established topic was greater than that for changing the topic, F(1,16) = 8.32, p < .05. Means are presented in Table 3. The interaction was not significant. (Some subjects' proportions were based on small denominators because they displayed only a few instances of a particular type. When these subjects were eliminated from analysis, the results remained the same.) on Joining conversations.-Focusing the dyadic and triadic conversations in which the infant joined into a conversational topic, a final analysis was conducted to determine differences in the characteristics of mother and sibling utterances followed by an infant turn and mother and sibling utterances not followed by an infant turn. Using the probability of infant response as the dependent measure, the full design for this analysis would be a 2 (age) x 2 (joint attention) x 2 (addressee) x 3 (speech act) mixed ANOVA, with repeated measures on the last three factors. Because of small frequencies in some of the cells, however, the overall

erage length and number of infant turns of the uninterrupted mother-infant conversations are compared to triadic conversations, the pattern of results did not change: (i) the average length of the triadic conversations (13.7 turns) remained greater than that of the mother-infant dyadic conversations across both ages (3.4 turns), F(1,16) = 34.6, p < .001; and (ii) the average number of infant turns in the triadic conversations (3.0 turns) remained greater than that in the motherinfant dyadic conversations at both ages (1.5 turns), F(1,16) = 7.9, p < .02. Thus, the shorter mother-infant conversations, with fewer infant turns, were not due to the siblings directly depressing the lengths of the dyadic exchanges by interrupting. Infant-involved Conversations The following three analyses concern only those dyadic and triadic conversations in which the infant participated (approximately 38% of all conversations at 19 months and 55% of all conversations at 24 months). These were of special concern because of our interest in the kinds of turns infants took and the factors influencing infant turns. took an average of Turn type.-Infants 21.9 turns in infant-involved conversations at 19 months of age; they took an average of 37.6 turns at 24 months of age. This analysis defined turns as the use of English words by infants because judgments of topicality were necessary. A 2 (age) x 3 (turn type) mixed ANOVA found no main effect of age on the mean proportion of infant turns in infantinvolved conversations. There was a significant main effect of turn type, however, F(2,32) = 13.71, p < .01. Mean values for turn types are presented in Table 3. A Tukey test found that the mean proportion of Join Topic was significantly greater than that of both Change Topic and Initiate Topic, and that the latter two were also significantly dif-

TABLE 3
MEANPROPORTIONS SUCCESS AND RATES FIRST (and StandardDeviations) OF INFANT
TURNS AS A FUNCTION OF TURN TYPE TURN TYPE

INFANT FIRST TURNS

Join Topic
.32 92 (.16) (.15)

ChangeTopic
.19 .70 (.09) (.36)

InitiateTopic
.11 (.11)

Mean proportiona ........... % Successfulb ................

Mean proportion of Join Topic, Change Topic, and Initiate Topic all significantly different "a from one another, p < .05. (Proportions are based on a range of 5 to 72 infant turns.) b Success rate of Join Topic greater than Change Topic, p < .05. (Successful Join Topic proportion is based on a range of 3 to 17 Join Topic attempts; successful Change Topic proportion is based on a range of 1 to 14 Change Topic attempts.)

Barton and Tomasello


0.60

525

0.5

0.40.3
0.2

0.1,

0.o

question

comment

request

speech act

infant-directed

other-directed

FIG. 1.-Mean probability of infant response as a function of addressee and speech act ANOVA could not be run. Hence, the analysis was broken down into two parts, one assessing the effects of the nonlinguistic factor of joint attention and the other assessing the effects of the linguistic factors of addressee and speech act, in each case in combination with the effects of age. The 2 (age) x 2 (joint attention) mixed ANOVA found no main effect of age on the probability of infant response. There was a significant main effect of joint attention: the mean probability of infants responding was greater when engaged in a JAE with the speaker (.23) than when not so engaged (.13), F(1,16) = 10.37, p < .01. The interaction was not significant. The 2 (age) x 2 (addressee) x 3 (speech act) mixed ANOVA found a main effect of age: the mean probability of infant response was greater at 24 months (.22) than at 19 months (.14), F(1,16) = 4.77, p < .05. There was also a significant main effect of addressee: the mean probability of infants' responding was greater when they were directly addressed (.24) than when they were not addressed (.13), F(1,16) = 18.50, p < .01. The significant speech act x addressee interaction, F(2,32) = 5.38, p < .01, however, indicates that only when a question was asked did addressee have a significant effect on the probability of infants' responding; Tukey's (a) test for unconfounded means confirmed this finding (p < .05).' In the cases of comments and requests, the infants were just as likely to respond when they were not addressed directly as when they were. This interaction is shown in Figure 1. No other interactions were significant.

Discussion
The current study attempted to document the dynamics of linguistic and nonlinguistic interaction in mother-infant-sibling triads. Three sets of findings are of interest. First, it was found that children as young as 19 months of age were able to engage in triadic joint attentional episodes (JAEs) and triadic conversations. This is the youngest age at which such interactions have been systematically documented, with the exception of Bruner's (1977) observations of a single 13-month-old infant learning to give objects to both his mother and an experimenter. Further, the proportion of triadic JAEs and conversations increased significantly from 19 to 24 months of age, indicating that important developments in these skills are still taking place during the

'Because some cells were still based on small frequencies (range 1-108), the analysis was repeated, deleting subjects who had any cell with fewer than five opportunities to respond. Results remained the same. Thus, the statistical values and data reported are for the entire sample of 18.

526

Child Development
tive effect of the triadic context on motherinfant dyadic interactions; mother-infantsibling triadic conversations are longer than mother-infant dyadic conversations. The reasons for this greater length cannot be directly determined by data from the current study. However, we may adduce at least some indirect support for the speculation that in a triadic conversation less pressure is put on each of the children. That is, during mother-infant dyadic conversations, roughly half of the conversational weight rests on the child; in these contexts the child has to take appropriate turns at all of the appropriate junctures if the conversation is to be maintained. In triadic conversations, on the other hand, when a child does not understand or have anything to say she may simply stay silent. The other two participants may continue the conversation, and the silent member might then rejoin later when she finds a comprehensible and interesting place to make a contribution. This interpretation is supported by the fact that mothers in the current study took about one-half of the turns in both dyadic and triadic conversations; each child took roughly one-half of the turns in their dyadic conversations with mothers, but only one-quarter of the conversational turns in triadic conversations. It seems plausible that such dynamics might account for longer conversational interactions in certain multichild contexts. One alternative hypothesis is that the triadic conversations in our study were longer because they provided the infants with their only opportunity to converse with older siblings, since dyadic conversations and interactions between the children were almost nonexistent. This might have made the triadic conversations more motivating for the infants and thus more likely to sustain their interest and attention. The main evidence against this hypothesis is that triadic conversations were longer, with more child turns, for siblings as well as infants. It is unlikely that interacting with the infants in this setting was especially motivating for the older siblings, however, as previous research for a similar context (i.e., the presentation of new toys) has shown that preschoolers prefer playing alone with the toys than conversing with their infant siblings (Tomasello & Mannle, 1985). It would thus seem most plausible to attribute the greater length of triadic conversations in the current study to differences in the basic dynamics of dyadic and triadic in-

second half of the second year of life. It is also important that mother-infant dyadic interaction in the current study did not change over time, and that sibling-infant JAEs and conversations were virtually nonexistent at both ages. Thus, all increases in the infants' interactions and conversations with their mothers or siblings in the triadic context during this developmental period were due almost exclusively to their increased involvement in triadic interactions and conversations. The second set of findings was quite unconversaexpected. Mother-infant-sibling tions were almost three times longer than either mother-infant or mother-sibling conversations, with all three participants taking more turns than they did in dyadic conversations. This was unexpected because previous research has reported that infants produce more utterances overall in dyadic contexts than in triadic contexts (Jones & Adamson, 1987; Woollett, 1986). More utterances does not mean longer conversations, however, and thus the increased length of triadic conversations presumably reflects a change in the dynamics of conversation when three as opposed to two people are involved. Our analyses of infant turns established that the increased length of triadic conversations was not due to some dynamic that excluded the infant. It was not the case that many triadic conversations reflected the mother's carrying on two essentially separate conversations with her two children. The vast majority of triadic conversations had some form of reciprocation between all participants, including the infant. Nor were the longer triadic conversations due to some depressive effect of the triadic context on dyadic conversations. Interrupted motherinfant conversations were not any shorter than uninterrupted ones, and, moreover, the dyadic conversations in the current study are of roughly comparable length (or slightly longer), with the same number of infant turns, as the mother-infant dyadic conversations of children of this age in purely dyadic contexts. For example, in a study of 10 1824-month-old later-born children, Tomasello and Mannle (1985) found an average conversation length of 2.6 turns for mother-infant dyads (3.8 in the current study), with an average of 1.3 infant turns (1.8 in the current study). Our conclusion is thus that the longer triadic conversations, with a greater number of infant turns, are not due to some artifact of our triadic coding scheme, nor to a disrup-

Barton and Tomasello


teractions. It must be kept in mind, however, that these dynamics only obtain when the participants have reached some critical level of linguistic skill, as evidenced by the fact that Tomasello et al. (1986) found that 1824-month-old twins (average age of 21 months) did not engage in triadic conversations with their mothers beyond a few very short episodes. The linguistic skills of these twins were somewhat less than those of even the 19-month-olds in the current study, which would seem to indicate that our subjects might have just recently attained the requisite level of linguistic skill necessary for lengthy triadic conversations. The third and final set of findings concerns the conditions that facilitate infant participation within the triadic context. Because joining a conversation (dyadic or triadic) is in some sense a more difficult task than initiating a topic in that it requires the infant to understand another speaker's topic, it was striking to find that nearly one-third of the infants' first turns joined into an ongoing conversation, and an additional one-third of their turns continued a conversation. In contrast, the presumably easier task of simply saying whatever is on one's mind (initiating a topic or interrupting to change the topic) comprised only 11% and 19% of the infants' first turns, respectively. This result suggests that the ongoing conversation offers important scaffolding for the infant by indicating a topic the speaker is interested in and therefore likely to continue. The success of joining a conversation indicated that this was precisely the case: 92% of the time infants joined a conversation, that conversation continued, while changing the topic was successful for infants only 70% of the time. Dunn and Shatz (1989) also found that infants were more successful when their intrusions were topic-relevant (joined) than when topic-nonrelevant they were (changed topic). These results thus confirm that children nearing their second birthdays are quite good at joining into an ongoing conversational topic, which presumably requires comprehension of that topic. Joint attention was clearly an important factor facilitating infant participation in triadic conversations. Infants of both ages were more likely to take a turn when they shared a joint attentional focus with the speaker, thus information indicating that nonlinguistic about the focus of interaction somehow facilitated their verbal contributions. Although in some ways this is an unsurprising finding, it nevertheless goes a long way in explaining

527

why infants are able to join into only some types of ongoing interactions among other participants. Thus, it is presumably the case that 19-24-month-old infants are not able to join into many adult-adult conversationseven those that contain simple languagebecause these conversations are not about perceptually available topics that can be specified nonverbally. In the current study with siblings and mothers and a new set of toys, on the other hand, they joined in readily. It may thus be that the concreteness of topics in mother-sibling interactions makes joint attentional interactions quite natural for the infant, which in turn makes possible her participation in triadic conversations. One implication of this, however, is that the 19-24-month-old infant's participation in triadic conversations may still be somewhat fragile, and thus it may not manifest itself in less propitious circumstances than those provided for them in the current study. Nevertheless, just how in tune infants were in the current context is indicated by the fact that they made relevant verbal contributions to comments and requests equally often whether or not they were directly addressed. (The fact that infants responded differentially to questions depending on whether or not they were addressed shows that this was not due to their inability to tell who was being addressed.) This result reflects in part the simple fact that when mothers make comments or requests for action, they often tend not to have any particular expectation that the child take a subsequent verbal turn-at least not as much as with questions. But, in combination with the findings that infants joined into ongoing conversations quite successfully and that joint attention facilitated infant conversational participation overall, it also implies that children younger than 2 years of age can process and respond to a substantial amount of language not directly addressed to them if that language is used in a comprehensible situational context. The findings of the current study clearly demonstrate that triadic interactions have their own dynamics that cannot be reduced to principles of dyadic interaction. Having three participants changes the demands placed on each participant and, in some circumstances at least, leads to more extended conversational interactions. Individual participants in these interactions are provided with numerous opportunities and some practice at joining into ongoing conversational

528

Child Development
distinction. Journal of Child Language, 10,
35-43.

interactions between other persons, which is simply not possible in dyadic interactions of any type. In addition, it may be that the mother-infant-sibling triadic context in particular, as one instance of a triadic multichild context, has some unique features that encourage infant participation in multispeaker conversations. In particular, the motherinfant-sibling context may facilitate infant participation more than triadic contexts with two adults because the conversational topics of adults often do not concern things that lend themselves to a nonlinguistic joint attentional focus among all participants. On the other hand, mother-infant-sibling interaction may also encourage infant participation more than triadic contexts with an adult and same-aged peer (including a twin) in cases where neither child is able to carry much of the conversational load. The unique features of mother-infantsibling interaction may or may not have important effects on infant communicative competence; we collected no data on this question. One suggestive finding, however, is reported by Vandell and Wilson (1987) who found that infant experiences with siblings predicted their subsequent turn-taking exchanges with an unfamiliar peer. It might be, therefore, that mother-infant-sibling interaction of the type observed here fosters the development of communication skills that serve as a bridge for young children's interactions with peers in which many of the same dynamics obtain, both in terms of having child interactants and in terms of multispeaker contexts. In "any case, the motherinfant-sibling context is clearly a rich and environment important language-learning for many of the children in most of the cultures of the world. It is thus clearly important that we go beyond the mother-child dyad to investigate this and all of the other contexts of interaction in which children acquire their linguistic skills.

Dunn, J., & Shatz, M. (1989). Becoming a conversationalist despite (or because of) having an older sibling. Child Development, 60, 399410.
Forrester, M. A. (1988). Young children's polyadic

conversation monitoring skills. First Language, 7, 145-158. Jones, C. P., & Adamson, L. B. (1987). Language

use in mother-child-sibling interactions. Child Development, 58, 356-366. Linton, M., & Gallo, P. S., Jr.(1975). The practical statistician: Simplified handbook of statistics. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Mannle, S., & Tomasello, M. (1987). Fathers, siblings, and the bridge hypothesis. In K. E. Nelson & A. van Kleeck (Eds.), Children's language (Vol. 6, pp. 23-42). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Nelson, K. (1973). Structureand strategy in learning to talk. Monographsof the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(1-2, Serial
No. 149).

Nelson, K. E., Baker, N. D., Denninger, M., Bonvillian, J. D., & Kaplan, B. J. (1985). Cookie versus Do-it-again: Imitative-referentialand personal-social-syntactic-initiating language style in young children. Linguistics, 23,
433-454.

References
Bakeman, R., & Gottman,J. M. (1986). Observing interaction: An introduction to sequential
analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ochs, E. (1982). Talking to children in western Samoa. Language in Society, 11, 77-104. Olson, S. L., Bayles, K., & Bates, J. E. (1986). Mother-child interaction and children's speech progress: A longitudinal study of the first two years. Merrill-PalmerQuarterly, 32, 1-20. Oshima-Takane, Y. (1988). Children learn from speech not addressed to them: The case of personal pronouns. Journal of Child Language, 15, 95-108. Rice, M. (1989). Children's language acquisition. American Psychologist, 44, 149-156. Schaffer, H. R. (1989). Language development in context. In S. von Tetchner, L. Siegel, & L. Smith (Eds.), The social and cognitive aspects of normal and atypical language development (pp. 1-22). New York:Springer-Verlag.
Schaffer, H. R., & Liddell, C. (1984). Adult-child

Bruner,J. (1977). Early social interaction and language acquisition. In H. Shaffer(Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction (pp. 271-290).
London: Academic Press.

interaction under dyadic and polyadic conditions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2, 33-42.
Schieffelin, B. B. (1979). Getting it together: An

ethnographic approachto the study of the development of communicative competence. In E. Ochs & B. B. Schieffelin (Eds.), Developmental pragmatics (pp. 73-110). New York: Academic Press. Schieffelin, B. B., & Ochs, E. (1983). A cultural

Della Corte, M., Benedict, H., & Klein, D. (1983). The relationship of pragmatic dimensions of mothers' speech to the referential-expressive

Barton and Tomasello


perspective on the transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication. In R. M. Golinkoff (Ed.), The transition from prelin(pp. guistic to linguistic communication 115-132). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Tomasello, M. (1988). The role of joint attentional processes in early language development. Language Sciences, 10, 69-88. Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 1454-1463. Tomasello, M., & Mannle, S. (1985). Pragmatics of sibling speech to one-year-olds. Child Development, 56, 911-917. Tomasello, M., Mannle, S., & Kruger, A. C. (1986). Linguistic environment of 1- to 2-year-old

529

twins. Developmental Psychology, 22, 169176. Tomasello, M., & Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Language, 4, 97-212. Vandell, D., & Wilson, K. (1987). Infant's interaction with mother, sibling, and peer. Child Development, 58, 176-186. Wells, G. (1981). Learning through interaction: The study of language development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Woollett, A. (1986). The influence of older siblings on the language environment of young children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 235-245.

S-ar putea să vă placă și