Sunteți pe pagina 1din 4

Journal of Memory and Language 53 (2005) 594628

Journal of Memory and Language


www.elsevier.com/locate/jml

Memory and the self


Martin A. Conway

The Leeds Memory Group, Institute of Psychological Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK Received 19 July 2005; revision received 29 August 2005

Abstract The Self-Memory System (SMS) is a conceptual framework that emphasizes the interconnectedness of self and memory. Within this framework memory is viewed as the data base of the self. The self is conceived as a complex set of active goals and associated self-images, collectively referred to as the working self. The relationship between the working self and long-term memory is a reciprocal one in which autobiographical knowledge constrains what the self is, has been, and can be, whereas the working self-modulates access to long-term knowledge. Specic proposals concerning the role of episodic memories and autobiographical knowledge in the SMS, their function in dening the self, the neuroanatomical basis of the system, its development, relation to consciousness, and possible evolutionary history are considered with reference to current and new ndings as well as to ndings from the study of impaired autobiographical remembering. 2005 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
` Keywords: Autobiographical memory; Episodic memory; Goals; Recollective experience; Deja vu; Amnesia; Neuroanatomy of memory; EEG; fMRI; Evolution of memory

A key feature of the approach taken to memory here, is that cognition is driven by goals: memory is motivated. This approach is embodied in a conceptual framework termed the Self-Memory System, (SMS, Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Conway, Singer, & Tagini, 2004) and the principal aim is to elaborate the nature of and rationale for the SMS. Findings from various domains are used to illustrate aspects of the SMS but a systematic review of the full range of ndings is not undertaken (several very detailed reviews are currently available; see, for example, Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Conway, Singer et al., 2004; McAdams, 2001, and edited volumes by Bluck, 2003; Hackmann &
q The author was supported by the award of a Professorial Fellowship from the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC), RES-051-27-0127 of the United Kingdom and he thanks the ESRC for this support. E-mail address: M.A.Conway@leeds.ac.uk

Holmes, 2004; Beike, Lampien, & Behrand, 2004; Skowronski, 2004). The SMS consists of two main components, the working self and the autobiographical memory knowledge base. When these components interlock in acts of remembering, specic autobiographical memories can be formed. Each, however, can operate independently and possibly enter into processing sequences other than those mediating memory. Both components and their interaction in remembering are considered in detail in this article. In the opening section, two general distinctions underlying the SMS framework are outlined. These are, rst, a distinction between the self-coherence of autobiographical knowledge and how it does or does not correspond to experience and, second, a distinction between very recent memories and long-term retention. Subsequent sections consider in turn, the working self, the autobiographical knowledge base, the construction of autobiographical memories, and the neuroanatomical basis of the system. The

0749-596X/$ - see front matter 2005 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005

M.A. Conway / Journal of Memory and Language 53 (2005) 594628

609

Fig. 5. Knowledge structures in autobiographical memory.

reviewed in Conway & Fthenaki, 2000). For example, most retrograde amnesics retain some conceptual knowledge of their lives and often this can be quite extensive. Even the most extreme patients, with no episodic memories, such as K.C. (Tulving, Schacter, McLachlan, & Moscovitch, 1988) and Clive Wearing (Wilson et al., 1995) retained some general autobiographical factual information. Other patients, such as SS (Cermak & OConnor, 1983), although apparently

having no episodic memories at all nevertheless had good knowledge of his past and a set of stories about various events which he retained in detail. Yet, other retrograde amnesic patients have temporally graded amnesia which often extends back to roughly the period of the reminiscence bump (when the patients were young adults, see Fig. 2) and may appear relatively normal for memories in this and earlier periods. (But note that temporal gradients do not take this form in

610

M.A. Conway / Journal of Memory and Language 53 (2005) 594628

Fig. 6. Episodic memories and a general event.

all cases and temporally graded amnesics are highly variable, Conway & Fthenaki, 2000.) Indeed, one notable patient whose retrograde amnesia extended back to when he had been 19-year-old, subsequently believed himself to be a 19-year-old naval rating on shore leave during World War II (Hodges & McCarthy, 1993). In the absence of all other autobiographical knowledge in what other way can the self-dene itself except in terms of knowledge currently available? Finally, it might be noted that in psychological illnesses too, access to the most specic types of knowledge, especially episodic memories, is attenuated. This is especially marked in clinical depression (Williams, 1996) but occurs in other illnesses too, e.g., schizophrenia, obsessional-compulsive disorder, etc. (see Williams et al., 2006, for a review).

Evidence from the laboratory and the study of memory disorders converges then on the view of the organization of autobiographical knowledge depicted in Fig. 5. One aspect of this organization which has been emphasized in the foregoing discussion is that subsets of knowledge and memories can be used to support and psychologically dene particular self-images. Recently, we have been studying current self-images and how these are related to selective sets of memories (Moulin, Rathbone, & Conway, 2006). In these experiments participants (n = 40, all middle-aged) complete a short questionnaire in which they list six I ams. . . (Kuhn & McPartland, 1954). They are instructed that an I am. . . can be anything and that they should respond with the rst six I ams. . . to come to mind and try not to edit or select. They then provide various sorts

M.A. Conway / Journal of Memory and Language 53 (2005) 594628

617

Fig. 9. Direct and generative retrieval.

Table 7 Schematized memory protocols Cue word Bicycle I can see my own bicycle at home in the garage I rode it a lot when I was home last year in the summer but not at Christmas because of the weather There is a pub on the canal near us and X and I cycled there and it was completely packed out with people sitting outside on the walls, everywhere(followed by series of detailed descriptions of buying drinks, seating arrangements, etc.) Seaside When did I last go to the seaside? I just had an image of a beach in Cornwall Im trying to remember going there on a holiday just after I left school, before college I remember we bought some nets, just like kids And now I remember a rock pool and we had out nets in the water, and our trousers rolled up, and we had a bucket too, I dont know where that came from(followed highly specic mainly visual descriptions of trying to catch small sh with her friends)

commence with an elaboration of the cue in terms of the organization of autobiographical knowledge, e.g., selection of a lifetime period or general event to search. Table 7 shows protocols for both these types of elaboration. Note that, on some trials there was no generative retrieval and a memory came rapidly and directly to mind. When generative retrieval was

present the initial elaboration of the cue was followed by further elaborations and in most cases retrieval was terminated when a set of vivid images (episodic memories) enter awareness. The number of elaborations in iterative searches that were undertaken was highly variable but over 80% terminated within ve elaborations (or at least as we measured these). There are some

S-ar putea să vă placă și