Sunteți pe pagina 1din 19

姝 Academy of Management Learning & Education, 2007, Vol. 6, No. 2, 167–185.

........................................................................................................................................................................

Understanding and Measuring


Linear–NonLinear Thinking
Style for Enhanced
Management Education and
Professional Practice
CHARLES M. VANCE
Loyola Marymount University

KEVIN S. GROVES
California State University-Los Angeles

YONGSUN PAIK
Loyola Marymount University

HERB KINDLER
Kindler & Associates

Various past studies compete and seemingly contradict one another in their presentation
of valuable alternatives to rational and logical thinking for improving decision making
and problem solving. We contribute here to an integration of past research and propose
and test a multifaceted construct of thinking style based on two primary dimensions:
linear thinking (e.g., rationality, logic, analytic thinking) and nonlinear thinking (e.g.,
intuition, insight, creativity). We present and validate in four separate studies a self-
report diagnostic instrument (Linear–Nonlinear Thinking Style Profile or LNTSP) for
measuring an individual’s linear and nonlinear thinking style profile. Validation study
results across student, professional, and managerial samples reveal a 4-factor model of
linear and nonlinear thinking style involving the manner in which individuals attend to
a particular kind of information source (internal vs. external) and subsequent linear
versus nonlinear processing of that information. The results also indicate that the LNTSP
has acceptable convergent validity vis-à-vis two commonly used and conceptually
related instruments, the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) and Cognitive Style Index, as
well as external validity and face validity across students from different academic major
programs (e.g., liberal arts vs. business and engineering), individuals from distinctly
different professional careers (i.e., professional actors and accountants), and managerial
and professional workshop participants. We discuss implications of these results for future
research, as well as possible beneficial use of the LNTSP for management education and
business practice.
........................................................................................................................................................................

Effective thinking, planning, and problem solving (Siggelkow & Rivkin, 2005). This reach must also
in our complex, turbulent, unpredictable, and un- include alternate thought patterns of intuitive and
certain global business environment require man- emotional assessments, creativity and lateral
agers and professionals to reach beyond merely a thinking, holistic and total systems appraisal, in-
linear thinking platform of rules, rationality, anal- tegrative and synergistic thinking, perceptual flex-
ysis, logic, reason, and cause– effect predictability ibility, imagination and visualization, and insight
167
Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the copyright holder’s
express written permission. Users may print, download or email articles for individual use only.
168 Academy of Management Learning & Education June

(Dane & Pratt, 2006; Sadler-Smith & Shefy, 2004; together to form an understood and predictable
Maani & Maharaj, 2004; Regine & Lewin, 2003; Zac- whole system. We continually search for new
caro, 2002; Sternberg, 2002; Buenger, Daft, Conlon, methods to objectively measure and predict the
& Austin, 1996; Csikszentmihalyi, 1996; Damasio, proportional cause-and-effect relationships of the
1994; DeBono, 1992). Although the importance of system components in the complex social and or-
these alternate patterns of thought has been em- ganizational phenomena around us (Waldrop,
phasized, their judicious use in tandem with more 1992; Wheatley, 1992). Extending this prevailing
traditional linear forms of thought is necessary to scientific perspective to management education
cope with the challenges of today’s business envi- and practice assumes that the relationships be-
ronment (Nadler, 2004; Hussey, 2001; Katz, 1997; tween variables are unidimensional and linear,
Senge, 1990). At present the conceptualization of presenting the rational goal of predicting and con-
these alternate patterns of thought to linear think- trolling human behavior and organizational out-
ing is fragmented at best. Their collective identifi- comes (Lichtenstein & Mendenhall, 2002; Capra,
cation and efficient measurement, along with pat- 1996). Extensions of the analytical methodology in-
terns of rational and logical thinking, could clude such cognitive processes of reason, logic,
provide potentially helpful diagnostic and devel- and rationality, still based on the assumptions of
opmental feedback for managers and profession- linear reductionism. However, this strictly linear
als interested in enhancing personal thinking style thinking style is less useful for understanding
flexibility and versatility in more effectively meet- complex and unpredictable nonlinear systems
ing the demands of our challenging business en- where the behavior of the whole is not necessarily
vironment. equal to the sum of the parts (Shelton & Darling,
The primary purposes of this research are to 2003; Waldrop, 1992).
develop a unifying construct for these alternate Despite the above predominance of a rational,
patterns of thought and integrate them with tradi- linear style of thinking, a distinction has long been
tional linear thinking style within a more compre- made between rational and nonrational human
hensive conceptual model for enhanced problem- thought (Sloman, 1996). Several theorists and re-
solving and decision-making effectiveness. In this searchers have proposed differing yet demonstra-
process we also present and test a self-assessment bly useful alternatives to this general linear think-
thinking style instrument that reflects a person’s ing style leading to dramatically enhanced
propensity to utilize linear and alternate thinking performance in viable idea generation, decision
style approaches. Finally, we discuss the implica- making, and problem solving (Dane & Pratt, 2006;
tions of this instrument, including the importance Sadler-Smith & Shefy, 2004; Skordoulis, 2004;
of balance and flexibility in its associated dimen- Runco, 2004; Peters, 1998; Csikszentmihalyi, 1996;
sions, for future research and practice related to DeBono, 1992; Segal, 2004; Nadler, 2004; Gibb, 2004;
management education and organizational perfor- Goleman, 1995). Some alternative perspectives em-
mance improvement. phasize the importance of intuition; others focus on
creativity and flexibility, holistic insight, and emo-
tion; and yet others involve a combination of these.
In Search of a Unified Counterpart to Linear
At first these alternative perspectives may appear
Thinking Style
as a motley array of thinking styles that are not
Thinking style has been defined as one’s preferred associated with linear analytic reasoning and
manner of using mental abilities to govern daily logic, or that merely share the common character-
activities, including understanding and solving istic of being “not linear.” However, research evi-
problems and challenges. At least partially social- dence suggests that they have much more in com-
ized, thinking styles may vary depending on the mon in their different but highly interrelated
conditions and demands of a given situation potential contributions to coping with the nonlin-
(Sternberg, 1997, 1994, 1988). The thinking style of ear dynamical nature and associated challenges
people in Western society is greatly influenced by of today’s organizations within the competitive,
a worldview grounded in the Newtonian reduction- uncertain context of our global business environ-
ist, determinist, and equilibrium-oriented tradition ment. These alternative thinking style approaches
in which any system is composed of divisible may help us in various ways to address the unique
parts. In this often-termed “linear reductionist” demands of highly complex nonlinear systems.
perspective relying on an analytical methodology We therefore argue that these heretofore appar-
(from the Greek “analyein” meaning “to break up”), ently diverse alternatives to a general linear think-
a system is equal to the sum of its parts that can be ing style dimension characterized by logic and
analyzed and understood separately and added reason can be appropriately grouped within a
2007 Vance, Groves, Paik, and Kindler 169

more comprehensive thinking style dimension of nonlinear thinking. Indeed, even critics of using
“nonlinear.” Although these various approaches intuition in executive decision making (e.g., Bona-
occasionally have been associated with nonlinear beau, 2003; Miller & Ireland, 2005) over traditional
thinking (e.g., intuitive and creative thinking often linear approaches and associated tools (e.g., deci-
are erroneously treated as synonyms rather than sion trees, system dynamics, agent-based model-
subsets of nonlinear thinking), and new nonlinear ing, etc.) acknowledge the need for intuition and
general thinking approaches and mental models other complementary nonlinear thinking forms.
increasingly have been called for based on con- Nonlinear systems are highly unpredictable,
ceptual applications of advanced mathematics where seeming chaos eventually emerges into
and new science theories of chaos, complexity, and new patterns of order (Holland, 1998; Gregersen &
quantum mechanics (Shelton & Darling, 2003; Sailer, 1993). As a fitting thinking style for nonlin-
Parker & Stacey, 1998; Horgan, 1989), we are un- ear systems, creativity is characterized by sponta-
aware of any concerted effort to build a case for neity and flexibility, with a balanced integration of
combining the above alternate perspectives much rational analytic and unconventional imaginative
more comprehensively under the unified, multifac- processes (Skordoulis, 2004; Runco, 2004; Katz,
eted construct of nonlinear thinking style. We now 1997), and where individuals in a highly focused
briefly examine four cognate approaches of intu- state of consciousness take new perspectives and
ition, insight, creativity, and emotion that often are reassemble interrelated parts of a system in novel
separately presented as counterparts to linear and unusual ways leading to viable solutions (Pe-
thinking, and consider how they may commonly ters, 1998; Csikszentmihalyi, 1996; DeBono, 1992).
contribute as interrelated forms of nonlinear think- The use of metaphors also can be helpful in in-
ing to better address the demands of a nonlinear creasing flexibility by bringing a comparison of a
world. problem with a seemingly unrelated object or sys-
tem, providing new perspectives for gaining better
understanding of complex systems, and generat-
Nonlinear Thinking: Toward a Comprehensive
ing multiple creative solutions (Aupperle & Kari-
Construct
malis, 2001; Aupperle, 1996; Tsoukas, 1991; Morgan,
Nonlinear dynamical systems are highly complex 1987).
and often rapidly changing, where a multitude of Insight is the result of a conscious and often
interrelated and interdependent parts contribute to frustrating unsuccessful rational analysis of vari-
outcomes that are very difficult to predict. Where ables of a complex nonlinear system comprising a
complex decisions need to be made under a tight problem, often followed by a period of inattention
deadline and in the midst of an overwhelming to the problem-solving task, at least in terms of
mass of information, successful managers often discernable activity (Yaniv & Meyer, 1987). Accord-
rely on intuitive judgment based on experience ing to some studies, this break in attention primar-
(Dane & Pratt, 2006; Miller & Ireland, 2005; Sadler- ily provides a productive diversion of attention,
Smith & Shefy, 2004). Defined as a holistic hunch or releasing the person from a restrictive mind-set
judgment derived from a subconscious synthesis of and erroneous set of assumptions and allowing
information across one’s diverse experiences, intu- fresh new perspectives and organizing assump-
ition has gained broad support for its utility in tions upon returning to the problem (Segal, 2004).
executive decision making. Holistic in nature, in- Other studies and several anecdotal accounts pro-
tuition is often the result of an automatic and un- pose an incubation phenomenon that automati-
conscious assessment of the interrelated parts of a cally follows in the break from the initial but fruit-
nonlinear system that scans the integrated “big less rational problem-solving effort, with an
picture” to point to appropriate decisions and new unconscious processing of information and data
directions, rather than getting delayed and lost in and their mutual causality in nonlogical and non-
the detailed analysis of a huge set of data (Clax- rational ways—more easily allowed by the sub-
ton, 2001). Miller and Ireland (2005) recently noted conscious— until a solution suddenly emerges and
that intuitive decisions involve “novel approaches, is realized in a “eureka!” moment (Gibb, 2004; Dorf-
changes in directions, and/or actions that run man, Shames, & Kihlstrom, 1996; Simon, Newell, &
counter to prevailing thinking or data” (p. 21), and Shaw, 1979). Whereas intuition yields a sense of
are often described as “gut feelings.” We argue direction and knowledge without a clear ability to
that effective decision making in a highly complex explain and to rationally justify (Miller & Ireland,
and turbulent business environment requires both 2005), insight ultimately is conscious and explica-
linear thinking and reliance on formal data ble—an enlightened solution to a previous state of
sources as well as intuition and other forms of quandary and uncertainty. And whereas intuition
170 Academy of Management Learning & Education June

may involve using integrated and holistic thinking be to develop an instrument that accurately mea-
on an unconscious basis (e.g., Sadler-Smith & sures these thinking styles. With this diagnostic
Shefy, 2004), insight is gained by initially examin- information, instructional and developmental ap-
ing in a consciously holistic manner as many rel- proaches may be designed, implemented, and
evant parts of a system as possible, eventually tested for building flexibility and capability in us-
followed by a rushing together of the pertinent ing these complementary styles. Consistent with
parts or variables in a conscious “big picture” of 2-part definitions from the literature of general
understanding (Nadler, 2004). thinking style and modes of thinking that involve
Considerable evidence exists that feelings and (1) attending to an information source, and (2) pro-
emotions can affect thinking and decision making cessing that information for subsequent decision
at unconscious and conscious levels, both of which making (Zhang, 2002; Sternberg, 1997, 1994; Stern-
are potentially useful to managers as they face berg & Grigorenko, 1995), we further define linear
complex challenges within a seemingly endless thinking style as a preference for attending to ex-
array of data and probabilities in nonlinear sys- ternal data and facts, and processing this informa-
tems (Sadler-Smith & Shefy, 2004; Goleman, 1998; tion through conscious logic and rational thinking
Damasio, 1994; Simon, 1987). Impaired emotional to form knowledge, understanding, or a decision
functioning can lead to continual search and anal- for guiding subsequent action. We also further de-
ysis of data and their myriads of permutations, fine nonlinear thinking style as a preference for
with the inability to make a decision in a timely attending to internal feelings, impressions, intu-
manner and move on by being “satisfied” with an ition, and sensations; and for processing this infor-
acceptable level of uncertainty (Goleman, 1995). mation (both consciously and subconsciously) to
The brain’s limbic system, much older in evolution- form insight, understanding, or a decision for guid-
ary development and playing a major role in hu- ing subsequent action.
man emotion, may exercise complete control over Although we are unaware of an instrument that
our actions before the younger and higher cogni- effectively measures an individual’s overall linear
tive centers of the brain are fully aware and take and nonlinear thinking style profile, we are famil-
conscious control (LeDoux, 1996). These feelings- iar with several instruments that have possible
based mental processes may scan an otherwise applicability. Likely the most well known, espe-
overwhelming presentation of data from a nonlin- cially in management education and development,
ear system and predispose an individual to focus is the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI). This in-
attention more deeply on a more realistic amount ventory assesses four personality types or orienta-
and arrangement of data. In addition, our con- tions (extroversion vs. introversion, judging vs. per-
sciousness of raised feelings, whether positive or ceiving) and four functions (sensing vs. intuition,
negative, resulting from a broad preconscious thinking vs. feeling). The four function dimensions
scan of the data also can influence our decision to relate most directly to our concept of linear versus
reject a proposed direction, to accept the direction nonlinear thinking styles, with external data sens-
and move forward with confidence, or to delay in ing versus intuition, as we have characterized,
the interest of gathering additional evidence. Fi- dealing with how people prefer to attend to or take
nally, recent work on appreciative inquiry and pos- in information, and the thinking versus feeling di-
itivity in organizations suggests that feedback and chotomy dealing with how people process and
other activities in organizations that promote pos- make decisions (Myers, 1998; Hirsh & Kummerow,
itive emotions can lead from moribund inactivity to 1998). However, recent studies have questioned the
confident and measured risk taking, realistic en- validity of the MBTI, particularly its criterion-
thusiasm, and creativity to more effectively ad- related and construct validity (e.g., McCrae &
dress a complex, chaotic, and turbulent world that Costa, 1989; Garden, 1991; Sipps & DiCaudo, 1988),
demands constant adaptation and innovation its use far beyond the original theoretical intent
(Losada & Heaphy, 2004; Cameron, Dutton, & and framework developed by Jung, and the poten-
Quinn, 2003; Cooperrider, Sorensen, Whitney, & tial confusion and interaction across scales related
Yeager, 2000; Seligman, 1998). to determination of dominant, auxiliary, tertiary,
and inferior functions and their conscious versus
unconscious states (James, 2003; Walck, 1996; Gard-
Measuring Linear and Nonlinear Thinking Style
ner & Martinko, 1996). In addition, with its focus on
If, as suggested earlier, the employment of both intuition and feelings, the MBTI presents a rather
linear and nonlinear thinking styles is a valuable limited range of approaches within the nonlinear
goal for effective management education and de- style dimension.
velopment, an important initial step would likely Additional measures somewhat related to our
2007 Vance, Groves, Paik, and Kindler 171

notion of linear versus nonlinear thinking style develop our own measure, the Linear–Nonlinear
include the Styles of Learning and Thinking (SO- Thinking Style Profile (LNTSP). The process that we
LAT) measure (Torrance, McCarthy, & Kolesinski, used for validating the instrument involved estab-
1988), Index of Learning Style (ILS; Felder & Silver- lishing the LNTSP’s psychometric properties, in-
man, 1988), Learning Styles Questionnaire (LSQ; cluding its factor structure, scale reliability esti-
Honey & Mumford, 1992), Kolb Learning Style In- mates, convergent validity estimates with well-
ventory (LSI; Kolb & Kolb, 2005), and Cognitive established and conceptually related instruments,
Style Index (CSI; Allinson & Hayes, 1996). The SO- external and face validity, and relative factor sta-
LAT instrument, originally intended to assess the bility across several student and managerial sam-
previously popular notion of right- versus left- ples. This thorough process is consistent with es-
brain dominance before further research dis- tablished procedures for psychometric test
counted the touted independence of the two hemi- validation (Hoyle, Harris, & Judd, 2002; American
spheres (e.g., Zalewski, Sink, & Yachimowicz, 1992), Education Research Association, 1999; Judd, Smith,
now is considered a useful measure for assessing & Kidder, 1991). The following section will detail
analytic versus holistic thinking modes (Zhang, four separate studies that involved developing the
2002). The ILS and LSQ include scales that tap LNTSP’s factor structure with a sample of univer-
dimensions similar to linear and nonlinear think- sity students, confirmatory factor analyses across
ing styles, such as sensing-intuitive and activists- a managerial sample, comparisons of the LNTSP,
theorists, respectively. Furthermore, the LSI is MBTI, and CSI across a separate business student
based upon experiential learning theory (e.g., sample, and a final study of LNTSP differences
Kolb, 1984; Kolb & Kolb, 2005), which states that across a sample of professional actors and accoun-
learning is achieved through the creative tension tants. Our overarching goal was to develop a rel-
among grasping experience modes (concrete expe- atively short, multidimensional thinking style
rience and abstract conceptualization) and trans- measure that (1) accurately reflects an individual’s
forming experience modes (active experimentation propensity for using linear and nonlinear ap-
and reflective observation). Although these modes proaches, and (2) can aid management educators
are also similar to our notion of attending to an and business professionals in their efforts to diag-
information source and processing such informa- nose and shape thinking style toward improved
tion for subsequent decision making, experiential managerial problem solving and decision making.
learning theory and the LSI comprise all human
adaptive processes such as problem solving, cre-
METHODS
ativity, and decision making, while the focus of our
model and measure is centered more specifically Study 1
on thinking style and its proximal impact on deci-
Sample
sion making. The CSI is a psychometrically sound,
unifactorial measure of the intuition-analysis di- The initial sample consisted of 293 undergraduate
mension of cognitive style, and has proved valid students at a medium-sized private university in
for differentiating general managers from entre- Southwestern United States. The sample repre-
preneurs (Allinson, Chell, & Hayes, 2000). However, sented a range of academic majors, including mar-
some empirical evidence (Hodgkinson & Sadler- keting (n ⫽ 44, 18%), engineering (n ⫽ 42, 17%),
Smith, 2003) suggests that analytical and intuitive management (n ⫽ 35, 14%), psychology (n ⫽ 20, 8%),
cognitive styles may not fall along a bipolar con- finance (n ⫽ 18, 7%), and international business
tinuum of information processing, as the CSI pos- (n ⫽ 18, 7%). One hundred forty-five students
tulates. In addition, as with the MBTI and SOLAT (49.5%) were female, and the ethnic breakdown of
instruments, the scope of the CSI is focused on the the sample was as follows: Caucasian (n ⫽ 125,
constructs of intuition and feelings and does not 43%), Asian-American (n ⫽ 52, 18%), Hispanic/Lat-
assess a broader assortment of nonlinear thought in-American (n ⫽ 35, 12%), African-American (n ⫽
or address the multidimensional nature of thinking 13, 5%), and Multiracial American (n ⫽ 18, 6%).
style. We concur with Hodgkinson and Sadler-
Smith (2003) and other scholars (e.g., Leonard &
Instrument and Procedure
Straus, 1997; Leonard, Scholl, & Kowalski, 1999) ad-
vocating relatively complex, multidimensional In developing the LNTSP instrument we first as-
theories and measures of thinking style. sembled 37 item pairs (74 total items) across two
Therefore, failing to identify an instrument that item sets in a forced-choice format similar to that
measures an individual’s composite picture of lin- of the MBTI. Linear items emphasized reason,
ear and nonlinear thinking style, we elected to logic, analytic thinking, deduction, and assess-
172 Academy of Management Learning & Education June

ment of external facts and data. Nonlinear items Analysis and Results
were associated with the previously described in-
Since the multidimensionality of the LNTSP had
terrelated approaches of creativity (e.g., spontane-
not been established by previous research, the fac-
ity, flexibility, open-ended), intuition (e.g., inner
tor structure of the instrument was assessed using
knowing, instinct, hunch); insight (e.g., bring to-
exploratory factor analysis (Gorsuch, 1983). Several
gether, all-at-once, experience, connect); and emo-
criteria were used to determine the factor structure
tion (e.g., gut feeling, heartfelt, empathy). The
underlying the LNTSP, including eigenvalues of
items were generated a priori by the authors based
greater than 1 and solutions that explain approxi-
on a review of related theory and research, and
mately 60% of the total variance (Hair, Anderson,
then presented to a 7-person independent expert Tatham, & Black, 1998). Principle component anal-
panel in a series of iterations involving item revi- ysis was utilized for factor extraction, and the Va-
sion, feedback, and face validity assessment. In- rimax rotation method was used to transform the
strument revisions to promote clarity also were final factor solution into a simple solution for in-
made based on feedback from executives and terpretation. Previous research has shown the Va-
other professionals attending workshops where rimax rotation to be superior to other methods for
the instrument was employed. Overall, the over- achieving a clear separation of factors and a fun-
whelming majority of workshop participants re- damentally simple factor structure (Kaiser, 1970,
ported that the items indeed measured one’s linear 1974).
and nonlinear thinking style preferences, thereby Beginning with the original 37 item pairs of the
establishing evidence of the LNTSP’s face validity. LNTSP instrument, principle component analysis
The first set of forced-choice items included 15 initially revealed a 4-factor solution. This result
pairs of statements describing alternative behav- was evident by a significant change in the ex-
iors. Using a Likert-type scale (3 ⫽ “very often,” 2 ⫽ tracted eigenvalues at four and five factors, as
“moderately often,” 1 ⫽ “occasionally,” and 0 ⫽ well as significant improvement in the total per-
“rarely or never”), respondents were asked to allo- centage of explained variance. We utilized the fol-
cate exactly three points across each pair of alter- lowing criteria to determine whether an item ade-
native statements according to how frequently quately represented a given factor: (1) a factor
they perform such behaviors. An example pair of loading of greater than .45, (2) item cross-loadings
statements included, “I primarily rely on logic on other factors that were less than .30 (Gorsuch,
when making career decisions” and “I primarily 1983), and (3) the theoretical contribution of the
rely on feelings when making career decisions.” item to a model of linear–nonlinear thinking style.
The second set of forced-choice items included 22 Given our primary interest in arriving at a brief
paired words or phrases that influence behavior instrument that may be readily used for manage-
and decision making. Using a Likert-type scale ment education, research, and practice, we set out
(3 ⫽ “very strong influence on how I behave,” 2 ⫽ to retain only those items that satisfied all three
“strong influence on how I behave,” 1 ⫽ “moderate criteria.
influence on how I behave,” and 0 ⫽ “little or no Overall, 26 items representing four factors met
influence on how I behave”), respondents again all three criteria above and were retained for fur-
were asked to allocate exactly three points across ther analysis (see Table 1). The first factor con-
each pair of alternative words or phrases. Example tained eight linear items, which generally re-
item pairs included “Feelings” and “Facts,” “Inner flected external sources of information, data, and
Knowing” and “Logic,” and “Felt Sense” and “Rea- influences that guide an individual’s decision
son.” making and behavior. The second factor also con-
All respondents in the first study were adminis- tained eight items, however of a nonlinear nature,
tered the LNTSP as part of their coursework. Stu- representing inner or internal information sources
dents were asked to participate in the study on a such as feelings, sensations, and impressions that
voluntary basis and were recruited from classes in influence an individual’s decision making and be-
their academic major. In an effort to make cross- havior. The third factor included five nonlinear
disciplinary comparisons to assess instrument ex- items that reflected the processing of internal
ternal validity, students were recruited from sources of information for the purpose of subjective
classes in business, engineering, psychology, and decision making and subsequent action. The
fine arts. Respondents were told that the LNTSP fourth and final factor contained five linear items
was a self-assessment instrument designed to that represented the mental processing of external
measure how individuals engage in decision- sources of information, including verifiable facts,
making processes. analytical reasoning, and objective factors, for the
2007 Vance, Groves, Paik, and Kindler 173

TABLE 1
Results of Exploratory Factor Analysis for Study 1

Factor Names and Items Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4

External Information Sources (EIS)

Concepts .65 ⫺.02 .18 ⫺.01


Rationality .51 ⫺.11 .07 ⫺.08
Reason .52 ⫺.21 .11 .06
Logic .62 ⫺.09 .10 .03
Facts .73 ⫺.18 .12 ⫺.15
Proof .56 ⫺.02 .20 ⫺.08
Data .71 ⫺.25 .09 ⫺.10
Deduction .51 ⫺.17 .07 .02
Inner Information Sources (IIS)

Instincts ⫺.04 .68 ⫺.07 .10


Empathy ⫺.08 .48 ⫺.13 .23
Felt Sense ⫺.04 .67 .02 .12
Inner Knowing ⫺.12 .68 ⫺.09 .13
Feelings ⫺.08 .51 ⫺.17 .17
Heartfelt ⫺.04 .50 ⫺.05 .18
Hunch ⫺.08 .56 ⫺.08 .28
Intuition ⫺.06 .46 ⫺.02 .12
Linear Decision Making (LDM)

I primarily rely on logic when making career decisions .15 ⫺.09 .53 -.12
I primarily weigh quantitative factors when making a decision about a large purchase .13 .05 .46 -.30
or investment, such as my age, budget needs, or future earnings
When making important decisions, I pay close attention to when a number of people .19 ⫺.01 .56 -.26
with well-justified expertise give me the same advice
The most important factor in making life-altering changes is knowing that the change .22 .09 .75 .01
is based on objective, verifiable facts
When my analysis and intuition are in conflict, I give precedence to my analytical .12 .01 .72 -.08
reasoning
Nonlinear Decision Making (NDM)

I primarily rely on my feelings when making career decisions .05 .12 ⫺.27 .56
I primarily weigh qualitative factors when making a decision about a large purchase ⫺.12 .09 ⫺.09 .67
or investment, such as my gut feelings or a sense that the decision is right for me
When making important decisions, I pay close attention when I experience a ⫺.01 .20 ⫺.25 .54
“knowing in my bones,” chills, tingling or other physical sensations
The most important factor in making life-altering changes (such as a career change, .06 .14 ⫺.16 .58
marriage, or major relocation) is feeling it is right for me
When my analysis and intuition are in conflict, I give precedence to my intuitive .03 .23 ⫺.03 .50
insights

Eigenvalue 3.56 3.54 3.23 2.45


% of Variance Explained 15.67% 15.61% 14.44% 11.43%

purpose of rational decision making and subse- source and decision-making processes (Zhang,
quent action. The eigenvalues were 3.56 (factor 1), 2002; Sternberg, 1997, 1994; Sternberg & Grigor-
3.54 (factor 2), 3.23 (factor 3), and 2.45 (factor 4), enko, 1995).
which explained 57.15% of the total variance. The The means, standard deviations, reliability coef-
factors were named external information sources ficients, and intercorrelations among the four fac-
(EIS), inner information sources (IIS), linear deci- tors are presented in Table 2. The external (EIS)
sion making (LDM), and nonlinear decision making and inner (ISI) information source dimensions
(NDM), respectively. This resulting 4-factor model demonstrated a significant negative relationship
comprised of two nonlinear factors (IIS and NDM) (r ⫽ -.47, p ⬍ .01), as well as the linear (LDM) and
and two linear factors (EIS and LDM) is consistent nonlinear (NDM) decision-making dimensions (r ⫽
with the previously mentioned 2-part definitions of -.43, p ⬍ .01). Furthermore, EIS was related to LDM
general thinking style involving information (r ⫽ .32, p ⬍ .05) while IIS was associated with NDM
174 Academy of Management Learning & Education June

TABLE 2
LNTSP Subscale Descriptive Statistics and Correlations for Study 1
M SD 1 2 3 4

1 EIS (8 items) 1.45 .44 (.87)


2 IIS (8 items) 1.74 .41 ⫺.47** (.84)
3 LDM (5 items) 1.68 .54 .32* ⫺.05 (.70)
4 NDM (5 items) 1.54 .55 ⫺.02 .29* ⫺.43** (.71)

Notes: N ⫽ 293; Numbers in parentheses are coefficient alphas.


* p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01.

(r ⫽ .29, p ⬍ .05). These results are expected in that dation studies on different groups, these results
the LNTSP presents respondents with forced- appear to corroborate the findings of prior research
choice items across the four dimensions such that on gender and academic program differences with
an individual’s preference for linear decision mak- respect to decision-making styles and preferences
ing produces a lower score for nonlinear decision (e.g., Abdolmohammadi, Read, & Scarbrough, 2003;
making. Thus, respondents with high scores on EIS Geary & Rooney, 1993; Ponemon & Glazer, 1990).
and LDM will obtain low scores on IIS and NDM,
respectively.
To examine differences between various subsets Study 2
of student respondents who are generally known to
favor linear and nonlinear thinking styles for the Sample
purpose of providing initial tests of external vali- A follow-up study was conducted with a manage-
dation, we conducted several post hoc analyses rial sample to cross-validate the exploratory re-
across the LNTSP dimensions. Consistent with pre- sults that were reviewed above. The sample con-
vious empirical research on gender differences sisted of 139 managers recruited from a large,
across emotional intelligence measures (e.g., multinational snack manufacturing company (n ⫽
Schutte et al., 1998; Petrides & Furnham, 2000; Ar- 29) and a fully employed MBA program composed
gyle, 1990) and social– emotional communication of managers from the aerospace, defense, telecom-
skills (Riggio, 1986), there were significant differ- munications, and financial services industries (n ⫽
ences between men and women’s scores across the 110). The sample included 84 men (60%) and 55
LNTSP dimensions. Specifically, women scored women (40%). The ethnic breakdown of the sample
significantly higher on the nonlinear LNTSP di- was as follows: Caucasian (n ⫽ 77, 55%), Asian-
mensions, IIS (F (1, 262) ⫽ 3.26, p ⬍ .05) and NDM (F American (n ⫽ 18, 13%), Hispanic/Latin-American
(1, 262) ⫽ 3.77, p ⬍ .01), than their male counter- (n ⫽ 17, 12%), African-American (n ⫽ 10, 7%), Na-
parts. In addition to gender, student major pro- tive-American (n ⫽ 1, 1%), and Other (n ⫽ 16, 12%).
duced significant differences across the LNTSP The mean age, years of overall work experience,
subscales. A comparison of business quantitative and years of managerial work experience of this
majors (finance, accounting, and computer infor- sample was 37.38 (SD ⫽ 10.74), 16.20 (SD ⫽ 10.14),
mation systems); engineering majors, and busi- and 8.86 (SD ⫽ 6.63), respectively.
ness qualitative majors (management, marketing,
and international business) produced significant
mean differences across the LNTSP dimensions.
Instrument and Procedure
Tukey’s honestly significant difference (HSD) test
indicated that business qualitative majors scored Managers from the snack food manufacturing com-
significantly higher on IIS (Mean Difference ⫽ .12, pany were administered a questionnaire consist-
p ⬍ .01) and NDM (Mean Difference ⫽ .16, p ⬍ .01) ing of the LNTSP and demographic items as part of
than engineering students. Furthermore, ANOVA a management development workshop. All 29
tests comparing engineering and marketing stu- managers attending the workshop agreed to par-
dents produced significant differences for EIS (F (1, ticipate in the study. Participants from the fully
84) ⫽ 3.55, p ⬍ .05), with engineering students scor- employed MBA program (n ⫽ 110) were recruited
ing higher than their marketing counterparts, through faculty member announcements in core
while marketing students scored significantly classes and follow-up e-mails requesting volun-
higher on NDM (F (1, 84) ⫽ 4.60, p ⬍ .05). Although tary completion of the questionnaire, which con-
preliminary and certainly in need of further vali- sisted of the LNTSP and demographic items.
2007 Vance, Groves, Paik, and Kindler 175

Analysis and Results TABLE 3


Measurement Model Parameter-Level Results of
Confirmatory factor analysis using Amos software
Confirmatory Factor Analysis
(Arbuckle & Wothke, 1999) was conducted to estab-
lish cross-validation support for the LNTSP factor Maximum Likelihood
structure results of Study 1. A measurement model LNTSP Factor Error Estimate
was created to assess the construct validity of the
External Information Source
four LNTSP subscales: external information
sources (EIS), inner information sources (IIS), linear EIS-1 .20 .66
decision making (LDM), and nonlinear decision EIS-2 .19 .77
making (NDM). In our postulating that each item EIS-3 .18 .80
EIS-4 .21 .71
would load significantly onto its associated scale, EIS-5 .10 .83
the measurement model was tested according to EIS-6 .35 .52
the following conventional indicators of fit: chi- EIS-7 .30 .67
square (X2), goodness-of-fit index (GFI), adjusted EIS-8 .23 .71
GFI (AGFI), root-mean-square of approximation Inner Information Source
(RMSEA), incremental fit index (IFI), and compara- IIS-1 .18 .70
tive fit index (CFI). On the basis of these conven- IIS-2 .21 .67
tional standards, the model demonstrated a mod- IIS-3 .23 .76
IIS-4 .25 .71
erate level of fit (X2 ⫽ 327 [df ⫽ 293, p ⬍ .01], GFI ⫽
IIS-5 .11 .85
.88, AGFI ⫽ .86, RMSEA ⫽ .09, IFI ⫽ .86, CFI ⫽ .85). IIS-6 .36 .57
Although prior research suggests that these results IIS-7 .33 .78
demonstrate a minimally acceptable level of fit IIS-8 .22 .73
(Kenny, 2000), Bollen (1989) asserts that fit indexes Linear Decision Making
greater than or equal to .85 are acceptable for mod- LDM-1 .31 .62
els, such as the present research, that attempt to LDM-2 .38 .70
open new directions in a substantive field. Table 3 LDM-3 .24 .88
LDM-4 .52 .77
illustrates the measurement model’s parameter-
LDM-5 .27 .52
level results, including the error terms and maxi- Nonlinear Decision Making
mum likelihood estimates for each item. The range
of maximum likelihood estimates across the four NDM-1 .35 .65
NDM-2 .45 .75
factors was .52 to .88, suggesting that the items NDM-3 .25 .86
loaded satisfactorily onto their respective scales. NDM-4 .49 .65
Overall, these results suggest that the factor struc- NDM-5 .32 .67
ture of the LNTSP derived in Study 1 demonstrates
reasonable stability across a managerial sample. Notes: X2 ⫽ 327 (df ⫽ 293, p ⬍ .01), GFI ⫽ .88, AGFI ⫽ .86,
RMSEA ⫽ .09, IFI ⫽ .86, CFI ⫽ .85.
Descriptive statistics, reliability coefficients, and
correlations across the LNTSP subscales are pre-
sented in Table 4. Similar to the results from Study ethnic background of the sample was as follows:
1, EIS and IIS factors demonstrated a significant Caucasian (n ⫽ 39, 49%), Hispanic/Latin-American
negative relationship (r ⫽ -.46, p ⬍ .01), as did LDM (n ⫽ 14, 18%), Asian-American (n ⫽ 4, 5%), African-
and NDM factors (r ⫽ -.42, p ⬍ .01). Furthermore, IIS American (n ⫽ 4, 5%), Native-American (n ⫽ 1, 1%),
was related to LDM (r ⫽ .25, p ⬍ .05) while IIS was and Other (n ⫽ 18, 22%).
associated with NDM (r ⫽ .28, p ⬍ .05).

TABLE 4
Study 3 Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Among
LNTSP Subscales for Study 2
Sample
M (SD) 1 2 3 4
In order to assess convergent validity estimates for
the LNTSP, another follow-up study was conducted 1. EIS 1.97 (.44) (.87)
with a sample of 80 undergraduate business ad- 2. IIS 1.16 (.46) ⫺.46** (.87)
ministration students in a medium-sized private 3. LDM 1.82 (.47) .25* ⫺.18 (.75)
4. NDM 1.29 (.49) ⫺.12 .28* ⫺.42** (.74)
university in the Southwestern United States. The
sample included 33 men (40%) and 47 women (60%), Notes: N ⫽ 78. Numbers in parentheses are coefficient alphas.
and the mean age was 20.55 years (SD ⫽ .83). The * p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01.
176 Academy of Management Learning & Education June

Instrument and Procedure Analysis and Results


Students were asked to participate in the study on To provide factor structure results from a second
a voluntary basis and were recruited by faculty cross-validation sample, we conducted a confirma-
members from business administration classes. tory factor analysis of the LNTSP using AMOS (Ar-
Participants were informed that the study was de- buckle & Wothke, 2003). Again postulating that
signed to help clarify how individuals engage in each item would load onto its respective dimen-
decision-making processes and the factors that are sion, the measurement model fit the data reason-
more important when making critical decisions. In ably well (X2 ⫽ 368 [df ⫽ 293, p ⬍ .01]) according to
order to demonstrate the LNTSP’s convergent va- the following fit indices: GFI (.93), AGFI (.89), IFI
lidity, participants were asked to complete a ques- (.92), CFI (.92), and RMSEA (.06). Furthermore, max-
tionnaire consisting of the LNTSP, dimensions of imum likelihood estimates across all four dimen-
the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI; Myers, sions ranged from .44 to .80, suggesting that each
1998), Cognitive Style Index (CSI; Allinson & Hayes, item loaded significantly onto its respective scale.
1996), and demographic items. The utilization of The descriptive statistics and correlations among
the MBTI and CSI was intended to measure the the LNTSP subscales, MBTI, and CSI are presented
extent to which respondents’ LNTSP scores are suf- in Table 5. The pattern of relationships among the
ficiently distinct from theoretically similar mea- LNTSP dimensions was similar to Study 1 in that
sures. The MBTI’s preference dimensions for gath- the EIS–IIS and LDM–NDM relationships demon-
ering data (Sensing or Intuitive) were measured to strated significant negative correlations. The pat-
assess the empirical relationships with the IIS and tern of correlations among the LNTSP subscales,
EIS dimensions. The MBTI’s preference dimensions MBTI dimensions, and the CSI suggests that the
for information processing (Feeling or Thinking) three measures share a moderately significant
were also measured to examine the empirical re- amount of variance. The average correlation be-
lationships with the LDM and NDM dimensions. tween the LNTSP subscales and the MBTI dimen-
The MBTI’s extraversion–introversion dimension sions was .30 and ranged from -.35 to .39. The rela-
was excluded from our analysis because of its em- tionships among the LNTSP and MBTI dimensions
phasis on an individual’s social interaction needs were in the expected direction, as EIS and LDM
and work environment preferences rather than think- were positively correlated with MBTI-Sensing and
ing style and decision-making preferences (Gardner MBTI-Thinking, while IIS and NDM were positively
& Martinko, 1996). Finally, the CSI was utilized for correlated with MBTI-Intuitive and MBTI-Feeling.
convergent validity estimates given its conceptual Similarly, the range of correlations among the
similarity to the LNTSP, specifically its focus on an- LNTSP subscales and CSI was -.35 to .37, and all
alytical and intuitive cognitive styles of information were in the predicted direction such that EIS and
processing. High scores on the unidimensional CSI LDM were positively associated with the CSI while
indicate a preference for analytical, logical, and IIS and NDM were negatively associated with CSI
data-driven decision making while low scores indi- scores. These results suggest that although the
cate a preference for intuitive, emotional, and feel- LNTSP dimensions share variance with competing
ing-based decision making. instruments that measure similar constructs,

TABLE 5
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Among Measures for Study 3
M (SD) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. EIS 1.48 (.38) (.81)


2. IIS 1.52 (.38) ⫺.45** (.81)
3. LDM 1.70 (.39) .32* ⫺.12 (.72)
4. NDM 1.30 (.39) ⫺.10 .30* ⫺.42** (.72)
5. MBTI-S 14.89 (6.38) .37** ⫺.35** .07 ⫺.10 ⫺
6. MBTI-N 10.93 (4.67) ⫺.30* .34** ⫺.17 .13 ⫺.86** ⫺
7. MBTI-F 9.80 (4.58) ⫺.25** .37** ⫺.35** .39** .02 .05 ⫺
8. MBTI-T 12.19 (7.47) .30* ⫺.20* .31** ⫺.37** .06 ⫺.10 ⫺.82** ⫺
9. CSI 43.67 (11.69) .31** ⫺.25** .37** ⫺.35* .48** ⫺.45** .05 ⫺.02 (.78)

Notes: N ⫽ 80. Numbers in parentheses are coefficient alphas.


* p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01.
2007 Vance, Groves, Paik, and Kindler 177

thereby providing evidence of convergent validity, more, maximum likelihood estimates across all
the LNTSP does not completely overlap such mea- four dimensions ranged from .49 to .83, suggesting
sures. Thus, the LNTSP subscales appear to tap that each item loaded significantly onto its respec-
unique dimensions of thinking style preferences. tive scale. The descriptive statistics and correla-
tions among the LNTSP subscales and the CSI are
Study 4 presented in Table 6. Similar to the results from
Studies 2 and 3, the external (EIS) and inner (IIS)
Sample information source dimensions demonstrated a
The final cross-validation sample consisted of 63 significant negative relationship (r ⫽ -.48, p ⬍ .01),
professional actors (n ⫽ 33, 52%) and accountants as well as the linear (LDM) and nonlinear (NDM)
(n ⫽ 30, 48%), who were targeted as an additional decision-making dimensions (r ⫽ -.40, p ⬍ .01). Fur-
sample given their expected differences across the thermore, EIS was related to LDM (r ⫽ .28, p ⬍ .05)
LNTSP dimensions. Specifically, we expected ac- while IIS was associated with NDM (r ⫽ .32, p ⬍
tors to score significantly higher across the nonlin- .01). Demonstrating additional convergent validity,
ear LNTSP dimensions (e.g., see Pinard & Allio, the range of correlations among the LNTSP sub-
2005; Moshavi, 2001; Bromage, 2000) while accoun- scales and CSI was -.29 to .34, suggesting that the
tants were expected to report a preference for the measures share a moderate amount of variance
linear LNTSP dimensions (e.g., see Abdolmoham- but appear to tap conceptually differentiated con-
madi, Read, & Scarbrough, 2003; Smith, 1999; Har- structs. As expected, the actors scored significantly
ris, 1994). The sample included 31 men (49%) and 32 higher on the ISI (t ⫽ 3.65, p ⬍ .01) and NDM (t ⫽
women (51%), and the mean age was 35.25 years 5.47, p ⬍ .01) than the accountants, while the ac-
(SD ⫽ 13.47). The ethnic background of the sample countants scored significantly higher on the EIS
was as follows: Caucasian (n ⫽ 37, 58%), Hispanic/ (t ⫽ 3.65, p ⬍ .01) and LDM (t ⫽ 5.47, p ⬍ .01) than the
Latin-American (n ⫽ 12, 19%), Asian-American (n ⫽ actors. Furthermore, the accountants scored signif-
3, 5%), African-American (n ⫽ 1, 2%), Native-Amer- icantly higher on the CSI (t ⫽ 2.47, p ⬍ .05) than the
ican (n ⫽ 3, 5%), and Other (n ⫽ 7, 11%). The actors actors, suggesting that the accountants preferred
(M ⫽ 12.43, SD ⫽ 11.94) and accountants (M ⫽ 8.52, an analytical, logical, and data-driven decision-
SD ⫽ 11.33) had been fully employed in their re- making style while the actors preferred an intui-
spective fields for a considerable number of years. tive, emotional, feeling-based decision-making
style. Overall, these results are very consistent
with those of Study 1, which found that students in
Instrument and Procedure the more quantitative accounting, finance, and en-
The actors were recruited through contacts made gineering academic programs significantly fa-
at the Southern California office of the Screen Ac- vored the linear LNTSP dimension compared to
tors Guild (SAG) and at two SAG-sponsored meet- “softer” management, marketing, and interna-
ings. Professional accountants were recruited tional business students, who preferred the nonlin-
through alumni contacts at large accounting firms ear LNTSP dimension.
in Southern California and through contacts made
through the Southern California Chapter of the Overall Analyses of Linear–Nonlinear Thinking
Institute of Management Accountants. The ques- Style Profiles
tionnaire for this sample included the LNTSP, de-
mographic items, and the previously used compa- While studies 1– 4 provide empirical evidence of
rable measure of thinking style, the Cognitive the LNTSP’s psychometric properties, they do not
Style Index (CSI; Allinson & Hayes, 1996), to further
establish the convergent validity of the LNTSP with TABLE 6
a professional sample. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Among
Measures for Study 4

Analysis and Results M (SD) 1 2 3 4 5

To provide further cross-validation support for the 1. EIS 1.54 (.58) (.92)
model, we conducted a final confirmatory factor 2. IIS 1.46 (.58) ⫺.48** (.92)
analysis of the LNTSP using AMOS (Arbuckle & 3. LDM 1.45 (.57) .28* ⫺.14 (.78)
4. NDM 1.55 (.57) ⫺.08 .32** ⫺.40** (.78)
Wothke, 2003). The measurement model fit the data
5. CSI 38.41 (13.89) .30* ⫺.22* .34** ⫺.29* (.88)
reasonably well (X2 ⫽ 267 [df ⫽ 293, p ⬍ .01]) ac-
cording to the following fit indices: GFI (.92), AGFI Notes: N ⫽ 78. Numbers in parentheses are coefficient alphas.
(.88), IFI (.91), CFI (.90), and RMSEA (.06). Further- * p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01.
178 Academy of Management Learning & Education June

describe the multidimensional nature of the par- dimensions (cells 6, 7, 10, & 11). In addition, it is
ticipants’ thinking style profiles and implications interesting to note that there are very few partici-
thereof. A series of simple post hoc analyses were pants who fit extreme mixed linear–nonlinear pro-
performed to describe all possible thinking style files of high EIS and high NDM (cell 4), and high IIS
profiles of participants across the four samples and high LDM (cell 13), contrary to what we would
(n ⫽ 575). Specifically, we calculated the quartiles expect if participant responses were simply ran-
across the mean scores of each LNTSP dimension. dom.
For example, the quartiles for the overall mean
LDM score were as follows: 1.43 (25th), 1.77 (50th),
DISCUSSION
and 2.00 (75th). The following descriptors were
used to characterize each level of LDM: 2.01–3.00 Our primary purpose in this research was to de-
(“High Linear”); 1.78 –2.00 (“Medium Linear”); 1.44 – velop and test a comprehensive thinking style
1.77 (“Medium Nonlinear”); and 0 –1.43 (“High Non- model that accurately reflects a person’s propen-
linear”). The same analyses were performed sity to rely on linear and nonlinear sources of in-
across the other three dimensions (EIS, IIS, NDM) to formation and processes to guide subsequent ac-
arrive at a matrix of all possible LNTSP profiles. tion. Our resulting model of linear and nonlinear
Table 7 presents all sixteen LNTSP profiles and the thinking style, supported by factor analyses and
frequency of each profile across the full sample multiple-sample comparison results, involves the
(n ⫽ 575). The results show that an almost identical manner in which individuals attend to a particular
number of participants possess an extreme profile kind of information source (internal vs. external)
(n ⫽ 170, 29.6%) as those reflecting a more bal- and the subsequent mental processing of that in-
anced, medium profile of scores across all dimen- formation. Furthermore, in the course of this model
sions (n ⫽ 169, 29.39%). An extreme profile is de- conceptualization, our purpose was to develop and
fined as either high on both EIS and LDM (cell 1) or validate an instrument that may be used to mea-
high on both IIS and NDM (cell 16), while a more sure an individual’s tendency to use linear and
balanced profile reflects medium scores across all nonlinear thinking styles. This validated instru-

TABLE 7
Frequencies of all Possible LNTSP Information Source/Decision-Making Profiles for the Overall Sample
(N ⴝ 575)

High LDM/Low NDM Medium LDM Medium NDM High NDM/Low LDM

High EIS/Low IIS Cell 1 Cell 2 Cell 3 Cell 4


High External High External High External High External
Information Source & Information Source & Information Source & Information Source &
High Linear Decision Medium Linear Medium Nonlinear High Nonlinear
Making (n ⫽ 85, 14.8%) Decision Making Decision Making Decision Making
(n ⫽ 32, 6.09%) (n ⫽ 6, 1.04%) (n ⫽ 7, 1.22%)
Medium EIS Cell 5 Cell 6 Cell 7 Cell 8
Medium External Medium External Medium External Medium External
Information Source & Information Source & Information Source & Information Source &
High Linear Decision Medium Linear Medium Nonlinear High Nonlinear
Making (n ⫽ 41, 7.13%) Decision Making Decision Making Decision Making
(n ⫽ 39, 6.78%) (n ⫽ 38, 6.61%) (n ⫽ 8, 1.39%)
Medium IIS Cell 9 Cell 10 Cell 11 Cell 12
Medium Internal Medium Internal Medium Internal Medium Internal
Information Source & Information Source & Information Source & Information Source &
High Linear Decision Medium Linear Medium Nonlinear High Nonlinear
Making (n ⫽ 7, 1.22%) Decision Making Decision Making Decision Making
(n ⫽ 29, 5.04%) (n ⫽ 63, 10.96%) (n ⫽ 46, 8.00%)
High IIS/Low EIS Cell 13 Cell 14 Cell 15 Cell 16
High Internal High Internal High Internal High Internal
Information Source & Information Source & Information Source & Information Source &
High Linear Decision Medium Linear Medium Nonlinear High Nonlinear
Making (n ⫽ 3, .52%) Decision Making Decision Making Decision Making
(n ⫽ 26, 4.52%) (n ⫽ 60, 10.43%) (n ⫽ 85, 14.8%)

Note: Profiles were developed by calculating the quartiles across the mean scores for the four LNTSP dimensions and designating
appropriate low, medium, and high descriptors.
2007 Vance, Groves, Paik, and Kindler 179

ment then would be useful for future research that approaches in their thinking styles. Furthermore,
examines the importance of linear–nonlinear our research provides initial conceptual and em-
thinking style balance—the notion that linear and pirical support for a more comprehensive and mul-
nonlinear thinking styles represent valuable com- tifaceted construct and measure of nonlinear
plementary approaches that together can enhance thinking style that overcomes theoretical and prac-
the quality of management education outcomes tical limitations of other instruments.
and professional practice, ultimately leading to
improved organizational productivity.
Implications for Management Education
The results of this multiple-study investigation
provide preliminary support for our 4-factor model Professionals in management education, includ-
of linear and nonlinear thinking style. Exploratory ing professors, human resource practitioners, and
and confirmatory factor analyses revealed a sim- executive development specialists, may utilize our
ple structure of four LNTSP factors that represent LNTSP instrument for a range of management ed-
our theorized components of linear and nonlinear ucation applications. Given evidence that manag-
thinking styles, consistent with past 2-part models ers and other professionals must apply both linear
of general thinking style involving information and nonlinear approaches to clarify objectives, un-
source and decision-making process (Zhang, 2002; cover hidden opportunities, systematically investi-
Sternberg, 1997, 1994; Sternberg & Grigorenko, gate and resolve stubborn problems, and reach
1995). Namely, linear thinking style can be mea- difficult decisions complicated by a variety of
sured by an individual’s preference for considering stakeholders (Nadler, 2004; Katz, 1997; Csikszentmi-
external data and facts and the processing of this halyi, 1996; Senge, 1990), the LNTSP measure may
information through rational thinking to guide provide such individuals with valuable insight
subsequent action, while nonlinear thinking style concerning their flexibility and degree of balance
can be assessed by an individual’s preference for in using linear and nonlinear thinking style di-
attending to internal sources, such as feelings and mensions. For example, the LNTSP measure may
intuition, and using inner processes such as feel- suggest that an individual utilizes linear and non-
ings, hunches, and insight to inform and guide linear thinking style balance and flexibility (e.g.,
subsequent action. We also found the participants cells 6, 7, 10 and 11 in Table 7 indicating medium
in our research to possess relatively strong think- levels of IIS, EIS, NDM, and LDM), or that the indi-
ing style consistency, with little tendency for indi- vidual tends to favor a linear or nonlinear thinking
viduals to have a mixed linear–nonlinear thinking style approach (cells 1, 2, 5 or 12, 15, 16). We assert
style profile (e.g., high internal source of informa- that when managers consistently favor one dimen-
tion and high linear decision-making approach, or sion over the other, they are not addressing the
high external source of information and high non- above highly demanding activities with full
linear decision-making approach). strength and flexibility, and organizational perfor-
We noted external validation support for our in- mance will inevitably suffer. The challenge for
strument in its ability to distinguish individuals professors and other management educators is to
who are generally known to possess relatively accurately diagnose individuals’ thinking style
high nonlinear thinking styles (e.g., women, mar- profiles and provide development opportunities
keting majors, professional actors) from individu- that address their less preferred thinking modality
als who are generally known to possess relatively to achieve a synergistic pattern that fully utilizes
high linear thinking styles (e.g., men, engineering the strengths of both styles.
majors, accountants). Furthermore, we established While linear and nonlinear thinking styles de-
preliminary evidence of the LNTSP’s construct va- velop through many years of use and are influ-
lidity vis-à-vis intercorrelations with established enced by traits and personality variables (Stern-
personality type and thinking style measures, the berg, 1997), they can be changed through conscious
MBTI and CSI. Our results suggest that while the and willful developmental effort to improve think-
three measures share a modest amount of variance ing style flexibility and versatility. In our work-
in thinking style, the LNTSP appears to tap unique shops for building thinking style flexibility and
dimensions of the information sources and pro- versatility, participants complete the LNTSP and
cesses that individuals utilize for making deci- are provided immediate feedback on the nature of
sions. We believe that our conceptual model rep- their linear–nonlinear thinking style profile. We
resents a distinctive approach to understanding then work with participants across three diagnos-
thinking styles and that the LNTSP provides poten- tic groups (linear-dominant, nonlinear-dominant,
tially useful diagnostic feedback on the degree to and balanced) to understand and practice comple-
which individuals tend to use linear and nonlinear mentary perceptual and decision-making pro-
180 Academy of Management Learning & Education June

cesses in specific organizational scenarios utiliz- scores across IIS and NDM, respectively. And while
ing a style that they normally would not employ. some researchers have criticized the use of these
For example, a group of managers for whom the ipsative, forced-choice items and exploratory fac-
LNTSP has indicated a predominantly linear think- tor analysis to extract a measure’s dimensions
ing style would be provided a vignette represent- (e.g., Baron, 1996; Cornwell & Dunlap, 1994), others
ing a realistic work-related problem situation, have successfully utilized such items and analysis
which calls for a nonlinear approach for optimal strategy to produce sound factors and control for
decision making. After participants individually biased responses due to social desirability (e.g.,
and collectively reach a decision for the vignette Wong, Law, & Wong, 2004; Chan & Bentler, 1993;
using their primarily linear approach, we present Saville & Willson, 1991). Furthermore, Chan and
several alternative nonlinear approaches for ana- Bentler (1996) have shown that maximum likeli-
lyzing the problem situation and generating solu- hood estimation in confirmatory factor analysis
tions. We also provide training in dealing with may be effectively utilized with ipsative data and
ambiguity, holding problems in abeyance for ef- that the resulting test statistics are consistent with
fective incubation, full use of the senses and feel- that of nonipsative data sets. More fundamentally,
ings for expanded perceptiveness, and relaxation in the present study the inherent dependence of
techniques for enhanced mental preparation to re- the two choices for each item is consistent with the
ceive insights. In subsequent vignettes we ask par- two general types (linear and nonlinear) of infor-
ticipants to practice using new nonlinear ap- mation processing systems in human beings pre-
proaches to problem solving and decision making. sented by the literature (e.g., Epstein, 2002; Goll-
For participants whose LNTSP scores point to a witzer & Bayer, 1999; Sloman, 1996), where a
predominant nonlinear style, we provide training decision toward a more linear approach consti-
in rational, critical thinking skills involving a sys- tutes a corresponding move away from nonlinear,
tematic process of analysis in problem solving. and vice versa. Furthermore, scores on the infor-
Participants practice solving various organiza- mation source dimensions identified in this study
tional vignettes using different criteria or lenses are independent of the scores on the decision-
for analysis—including technical, economic, so- making dimensions, since it is possible, for exam-
cial–psychological, political, and legal– ethical ple, for individuals high in IIS or EIS to also be high
perspectives—and systematically applying each in LDM or NDM (refer to Table 7). Thus, despite the
lens for analysis before coming to a solution for the ipsative nature of our instrument, the exploratory
vignette. We also involve in the aforementioned results of Study 1 and confirmatory results of Stud-
training those participants for whom the LNTSP ies 2, 3, and 4 appear to provide preliminary sup-
instrument demonstrates a balance in linear and port for the LNTSP’s factor structure.
nonlinear thinking to increase their self-aware- In addition, the present study did not address the
ness, help maintain and reinforce this balance, LNTSP’s predictive validity or its incremental va-
and model their thinking style flexibility for the lidity beyond that of competing measures. More-
benefit and learning of other participants. In fact, over, the LNTSP’s predictive validity should be es-
we have found that through effective sharing and tablished vis-à-vis behavioral performance
interaction among participants in our training pro- outcomes to assess the dimensions’ proximal effect
cess, those with predominant nonlinear styles can on actual performance measures. Future research
help the linear thinkers build greater awareness would do well to establish the percentage of
and thinking style flexibility, and vice versa. The unique variance that the LNTSP explains in effec-
active participant interaction and sharing also tive managerial behavior beyond the MBTI, CSI,
clearly lead to a greater affirmation and appreci- and other factors, such as cognitive ability, mana-
ation for others possessing a predominant comple- gerial experience, and leadership style. Also, the
mentary thinking style. present study did not address the LNTSP’s consis-
tency over time; thus, future investigations need to
compare respondents’ scores over time to establish
Limitations and Future Research
test–retest reliability estimates. Finally, future
As with any preliminary validation study, there LNTSP research would benefit from cross-cultural
are a number of limitations that should be consid- studies to further establish the measure’s external
ered and addressed in future LNTSP research. validity and stability across diverse populations.
First, the use of forced-choice item pairs may have There are a range of very interesting and poten-
introduced method bias among the LNTSP factors tially fruitful avenues for future research on the
in that an individual’s high or low scores across LNTSP and linear–nonlinear thinking styles. First,
EIS and LDM produce corresponding low or high given the basic assertion here that a flexible and
2007 Vance, Groves, Paik, and Kindler 181

balanced use of both linear and nonlinear thinking ship theory and research has addressed the
styles is associated with effective professional per- efficacy of transformational and charismatic lead-
formance in organizations, future concurrent valid- ership models (Dumdum, Lowe, & Avolio, 2002), as
ity research using the LNTSP should examine the well as the relationship between social– emotional
link between level of linear–nonlinear thinking intelligence and transformational– charismatic
style balance (e.g., high linear vs. balanced linear– leadership behavior (e.g., Sosik & Dworakivsky,
nonlinear vs. high nonlinear thinking style) and 1998; Cooper & Sawaf, 1996). However, the in-
various measures of professional and managerial creased attention devoted to examining emotional
effectiveness. The predictive validity of the LNTSP intelligence as a predictor of exemplary leader-
and the underlying notion of linear–nonlinear ship suggests that the field of leadership theory
thinking style balance also could be tested by and research may need a new look at the corre-
measuring new employee thinking style balance lates of effective leadership. We recommend that
and assessing the instrument’s ability to predict future research on leadership behavior, transfor-
future employee success measures, such as sales mational or otherwise, examine the relative contri-
or overall performance appraisal scores. bution of balanced and flexible linear and nonlin-
In addition, future studies may pursue the pos- ear thinking as a strong predictor of exemplary
sible link between an individual’s flexible reliance leadership, as suggested by Rowe’s (2001) model of
on both linear and nonlinear sources of informa- “strategic leadership,” involving a balance of vi-
tion and decision-making processes and perfor- sionary (i.e., nonlinear) and managerial (i.e., lin-
mance in such critical areas as “moral imagina- ear) leadership efforts for creating value in orga-
tion” (Werhane, 1999; Ciulla, 1998), ethical or nizations.
socially responsible behavior (Groves, Vance, The LNTSP may have important implications for
Paik, & Kindler, 2005; Klein, 2002; Pennino, 2002), enhancing creativity and innovation in organiza-
and strategic management (Miller & Ireland, 2005). tions and requires further study. Effective creative
Given the recently publicized accounting scandals performance may rely primarily upon nonlinear
in many U.S. businesses, ethical behavior is a par- thinking skills, but the most effective problem-
ticularly poignant issue for managers and execu- solving behavior might require both linear and
tives in critical decision-making positions as well nonlinear thinking skills for testing and evaluat-
as business school faculty and professionals in ing creative alternatives for selection (Nadler, 2004;
management education. Also, further research is Katz, 1997; Csikszentmihalyi, 1996). In addition, be-
needed to understand the importance of balanced sides creative idea generation, the effective calcu-
linear–nonlinear thinking style according to man- lated and planned implementation of creative
agement level and an individual’s career progres- ideas and directions for organizational innovation
sion. Indeed, Katz’s (1974) popular model of three and change might also require complementary lin-
core managerial skills, which include technical, ear skills (Rowe, 2001). Furthermore, there are use-
human or interpersonal, and conceptual, suggests ful research applications at the group and organi-
that managers at the highest organizational levels zation level. To create a context or working
require mostly conceptual and human skills, while environment that effectively supports creativity
lower level managers require mostly technical and and innovation, a workforce that is composed of
human skills. More recently, empirical research individuals with strong thinking style balance
has supported the notion that significantly higher might provide a more supportive context than one
levels of intuitive, nonlinear thinking styles are where some employees are high in linear thinking
associated with greater levels of seniority (e.g., while others are high in nonlinear thinking—yet
Allinson & Hayes, 1996), while lower level employ- still on average reflecting a similar thinking style
ees are characterized by greater levels of linear, balance across the workforce.
analytical thinking (e.g., Hodgkinson & Sadler- Finally, where the value of linear–nonlinear
Smith, 2003). The LNTSP instrument may be used to thinking style balance and flexibility is clearly
assess the extent to which conceptual thinking, demonstrated by subsequent research in various
strategic thinking, and decision making require an educational and organizational contexts, an addi-
effective balance of linear (external) and nonlinear tional compelling prescriptive research direction
(internal) sources of information and thought pro- would be on methods of management education
cesses. and training that are the most effective in building
Another particularly interesting avenue of future linear skills or building nonlinear skills—which-
research is the value of flexible and balanced lin- ever is diagnosed by the LNTSP as being low—
ear–nonlinear thinking styles for effective leader- toward greater balance and flexible use. There
ship behavior. Much of the last 20 years of leader- might also be differing optimal developmental ap-
182 Academy of Management Learning & Education June

proaches for the different dimensions within each Chan, W., & Bentler, P. 1996. Covariance structure analysis of
style. For example, within the nonlinear style, the partially additive ipsative data using restricted maximum
likelihood estimation. Multivariate Behavioral Research,
most effective approach for developing creative 31(3): 289 –312.
thinking skills may differ from the most effective
Ciulla, J. B. 1998. Imagination, fantasy, wishful thinking, and
approach for developing inner feeling aware- truth. Business Ethics Quarterly, Special Issue #1: 99 –107.
ness. Thus, subsequent research on the most
Claxton, G. 2001. The anatomy of intuition. In T. Atkinson & G.
cost-effective approaches for achieving linear–
Claxton (Eds.), The intuitive practitioner: 32-52. Bucking-
nonlinear thinking style balance and flexibility ham, UK: Open University Press.
could be very beneficial.
Cooper, R. K., & Sawaf, A. 1996. Executive EQ: Emotional intel-
ligence in leadership and organizations. New York: Gros-
set/Putnam.
REFERENCES
Cooperrider, D., Sorensen, P., Whitney, D., & Yeager, T. (Eds.)
Abdolmohammadi, M. J., Read, W. J., & Scarbrough, D. P. 2003. 2000. Appreciative inquiry: Rethinking human organization
Does selection-socialization help to explain accountants’ toward a positive theory of change. Champaign, IL: Stipes
weak ethical reasoning? Journal of Business Ethics, 42(1): Publishing.
71– 81.
Cornwell, J., & Dunlap, W. 1994. On the questionable soundness
Allinson, A.W., Chell, E., & Hayes, J. 2000. Intuition and entre- of factoring ipsative data: A response to Saville & Willson
preneurial behaviour. European Journal of Work and Orga- (1991). Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychol-
nizational Psychology, 9(1): 31– 43. ogy, 67: 89 –100.
Allinson, A.W., & Hayes, J. 1996. The Cognitive Style Index: A Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1996. Creativity: Flow and the psychology
measure of intuition-analysis for organizational research. of discovery and invention. New York: HarperCollins Pub-
Journal of Management Studies, 33: 119 –135. lishers.
American Education Research Association. 1999. Standards in Damasio, A. R. 1994. Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the
educational and psychological testing: American Education human brain. New York: HarperCollins Publishers.
Research Association (AERA), American Psychological As-
sociation (APA), & National Council on Measurement in Dane, E., & Pratt, M. G. 2006. Exploring intuition and its role in
Education (NCME). Washington, DC: American Education managerial decision-making. Academy of Management
Research Association. Review (in press).

Arbuckle, J., & Wothke, W. 1999. AMOS 4 user’s reference guide. DeBono, E. 1992. Serious creativity: Using the power of lateral
Chicago, IL: Smallwaters Corporation. thinking to create new ideas. New York: HarperCollins Pub-
lishers.
Argyle, M. 1990. The psychology of interpersonal behavior. Har-
mondsworth, UK: Penguin. Dorfman, J., Shames, V. A., & Kihlstrom, J. F. 1996. Intuition,
incubation and insight: Implicit cognition in problem solv-
Aupperle, K. E. 1996. Spontaneous organisational reconfigura- ing. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Implicit cognition: 257–296. Ox-
tion: A historical example based on Xenophon’s Anabasis. ford: Oxford University Press.
Organization Science, 7(4): 445– 460.
Dumdum, U. R., Lowe, K. B., & Avolio, B. J. 2002. A meta-analysis
Aupperle, K. E., & Karimalis, G. N. 2001. Using metaphors to of transformational and transactional leadership correlates
facilitate cooperation and resolve conflict: Examining the of effectiveness and satisfaction: An update and extension.
case of Disneyland Paris. Journal of Change Management, Transformational and Charismatic Leadership, 2: 35– 66.
2(1): 23–32.
Epstein, S. 2002. Cognitive-experiential self-theory of personal-
Baron, H. 1996. Strengths and limitations of ipsative measure- ity. In T. Millon & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Comprehensive hand-
ment. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychol- book of psychology, Vol. 5: 159 –184, Personality and social
ogy, 69: 49 –56. psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Bollen, K. A. 1989. Structural equations with latent variables. Felder, R. M., & Silverman, L. K. 1988. Learning styles and teach-
New York: Wiley. ing styles in engineering education. Engineering Educa-
Bonabeau, E. 2003. Don’t trust your gut. Harvard Business Re- tion, 78: 674 – 681.
view, May: 116 –123. Garden, A. 1991. Unresolved issues with the Meyers-Briggs Type
Bromage, N. 2000. No business without show business. Manage- Indicator. Journal of Psychological Type, 22: 3–14.
ment Accounting, 78(3): 52–53. Gardner, W. L., & Martinko, M. J. 1996. Using the Myers-Briggs
Buenger, V., Daft, R. L., Conlon, E. J., & Austin, J. 1996. Competing Type Indicator to study managers: A literature review and
values in organizations: Contextual influences and struc- research agenda. Journal of Management, 22: 45– 82.
tural consequences. Organization Science, 7(5): 557–576.
Geary, W., & Rooney, C. 1993. Designing accounting education
Cameron, K. S., Dutton, J. E., & Quinn, R. E. 2003. Foundations of to achieve balanced intellectual development. Issues in
positive organizational scholarship. In K. S. Cameron, J. E. Accounting Education, 8(Spring): 60 –70.
Dutton, & R. E. Quinn (Eds.), Positive Organizational Schol-
Gibb, S. 2004. Imagination, creativity, and HRD: An aesthetic
arship: 3–13. San Francisco: Berrett-Koehler.
perspective. Human Resource Development Review, 3(1):
Capra, F. 1996. The web of life. New York: Anchor Books. 53–74.
Chan, W., & Bentler, P. 1993. The covariance structure. Sociolog- Goleman, D. 1995. Emotional intelligence: Why it can matter
ical Methods & Research, 22(2): 214 –247. more than IQ. New York: Bantam Books.
2007 Vance, Groves, Paik, and Kindler 183

Goleman, D. 1998. Emotional intelligence at work. New York: Kolb (Ed.), Experiential learning: 20 –38. Englewood Cliffs,
Bantam. NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Gollwitzer, P., & Bayer, U. 1999. Deliberative versus implemen- Kolb, A. Y., & Kolb, D. A. 2005a. The Kolb learning style inven-
tal mindsets in the control of action. In S. Chaiken & Y. tory—version 3.1: Technical specifications. Boston, MA: Hay
Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology: Resources Direct.
403– 422. New York: Guilford Press.
Kolb, A. Y., & Kolb, D. A. 2005b. Learning styles and learning
Gorsuch, R. L. 1983. Factor analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. spaces: Enhancing experiential learning in higher educa-
tion. Academy of Management Learning & Education, 4(2):
Gregersen, H., & Sailer, L. 1993. Chaos theory and its implica-
193–212.
tions for social science research. Human Relations, 46(7):
777– 802. LeDoux, J. 1996. The emotional brain. New York: Touchstone/
Simon & Schuster.
Groves, K., Vance, C., & Paik, Y., & Kindler, H. 2005. Linking
linear/nonlinear thinking style balance and managerial Leonard, D., Scholl, R. W., & Kowalski, K. B. 1999. Information
ethical decision-making. Paper presented at the annual processing style and decision-making. Journal of Organi-
meeting of the Academy of Management, Honolulu, HI. zational Behavior, 20: 407– 420.
Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. 1998. Leonard, D., & Straus, S. 1997. Putting your company’s whole
Multivariate data analysis (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: brain to work. Harvard Business Review, July/August: 111–
Prentice Hall. 121.
Harris, P. 1994. Can management accountants make decisions? Lichtenstein, B., & Mendenhall, M. E. 2002. Non-linearity and
Management Accounting, 72(7): 14. response-ability: Emergent order in 21st century careers.
Human Relations, 55(1): 3–32.
Hirsh, S., & Kummerow, J. 1998. Introduction to type in organi-
zations. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc. Losada, M., & Heaphy, E. 2004. The role of positivity and con-
nectivity in the performance of business teams: A nonlinear
Hodgkinson, G., & Sadler-Smith, E. 2003. Complex or unitary? A
dynamics model. The American Behavioral Scientist, 47(6):
critique and re-assessment of the Allinson-Hayes Cognitive
740 –765.
Style Index. Journal of Occupational and Organizational
Psychology, 76: 243–262. Maani, K. E., & Maharaj, V. 2004. Links between systems think-
ing and complex decision making. System Dynamics Re-
Holland, J. 1998. Emergence: From chaos to order. Redwood City,
view, 20(1): 21– 48.
CA: Addison-Wesley.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P.T. 1989. Reinterpreting the Meyers-
Honey, P., & Mumford, A. 1992. The manual of learning styles
Briggs Type Indicator from the perspective of the five factor
(3rd ed.). Maidenhead, Berkshire, UK: Peter Honey Publica-
model of personality. Journal of Personality, 57: 17– 40.
tions.
Miller, C., & Ireland, D. 2005. Intuition in strategic decision
Horgan, J. 1989. Nonlinear thinking. Scientific American, 260(6):
making: Friend or foe in the fast-paced 21st century. Acad-
26 –27.
emy of Management Executive, 19(1): 19 –30.
Hoyle, R. H., Harris, M. J., & Judd, C. M. 2002. Research methods
Morgan, G. 1987. Images of organization, (2nd ed.). Newbury
in social relations. Fort Worth, TX: Wadsworth.
Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Hussey, D. 2001. Creative strategic thinking and the analytical
Moshavi, D. 2001. “Yes and ” . . . : Introducing improvisational
process: Critical factors for strategic success. Strategic
theatre techniques to the management classroom. Journal
Change, 10(4): 201–213.
of Management Education, 25(4): 437– 449.
James, M. 2003. Using the Myers-Briggs type indicator as a tool
Myers, I. B. 1998. Introduction to type. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
for leadership development? Apply with caution. Journal of
Psychologists Press, Inc.
Leadership & Organizational Studies, 10(1): 68.
Nadler, G. 2004. Taking a holistic path. Industrial Management,
Judd, C. M., Smith, E. R., & Kidder, L. H. 1991. Research methods
46(6): 26 –31.
in social relations. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanov-
ich, Inc. Parker, D., & Stacey, R. 1998. Chaos management and econom-
ics: The implications of non-linear thinking. London: The
Kaiser, H. F. 1970. A second-generation little jiffy. Psy-
Institute of Economic Affairs.
chometrika, 35: 410 – 415.
Pennino, C. 2002. Is decision style related to moral development
Kaiser, H. F. 1974. Little jiffy, mark IV. Educational & Psychology
among managers in the U.S.? Journal of Business Ethics, 41:
Measurement, 34: 111–117.
337–347.
Katz, R. L. 1974. Skills of an effective administrator. Harvard
Peters, T. F. 1998. How creative engineers think. Civil Engineer-
Business Review, Sept./Oct.: 90 –102.
ing, 68(3): 48 –51.
Katz, A. 1997. Creativity in the cerebral hemispheres. In M.A.
Petrides, V., & Furnham, A. 2000. Gender differences in mea-
Runco (Ed.), Creativity research handbook: 203–206.
sured and self-estimated trait emotional intelligence. Sex
Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.
Roles, 6: 449 – 461.
Kenny, D. A. 2000. Measuring model fit. Retrieved from www.ad-
Pinard, M. C., & Allio, R. J. 2005. Innovations in the classroom:
venergy.com/⬃dakenny/causalm.htm.
Improving the creativity of MBA students. Strategy & Lead-
Klein, S. 2002. The head, the heart, and business virtues. Journal ership, 33(1): 49 –51.
of Business Ethics, 39: 347–359.
Poneman, L., & Glazer, A. 1990. Accounting education and eth-
Kolb, D. A. 1984. The process of experiential learning. In D. A. ical development: The influence of liberal learning on stu-
184 Academy of Management Learning & Education June

dents and alumni in accounting practice. Issues in Account- Sosik, J. J., & Dworakivsky, A. C. 1998. Self-concept based as-
ing Education, 5(2): 195–208. pects of the charismatic leader: More than meets the eye.
Leadership Quarterly, 9: 503–526.
Regine, B., & Lewin, R. 2003. Third possibility leaders: The in-
visible edge women have in complex organizations. The Sternberg, R. J. 1988. Mental self-government: A theory of intel-
Learning Organization, 10(6): 347–352. lectual styles and their development. Human Development,
31: 197–224.
Riggio, R. E. 1986. Assessment of basic social skills. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 51: 649 – 660. Sternberg, R. J. 1994. Thinking styles: Theory and assessment at
the interface between intelligence and personality. In R. J.
Rowe, W. G. 2001. Creating wealth in organizations: The role of
Sternberg & P. Ruzgis (Eds.), Intelligence and personality:
strategic leadership. Academy of Management Executive,
169 –187. New York: Cambridge University Press.
15(1): 81–94.
Sternberg, R. J. 1997. Thinking styles. New York: Cambridge
Runco, M. A. 2004. Creativity. Annual Review of Psycyhology, 55: University Press.
657– 687.
Sternberg, R. J. 2002. Successful intelligence: A new approach to
Sadler-Smith, E., & Shefy, E. 2004. The intuitive executive: Un- leadership. In R. E. Riggio, S. E. Murphy, & F. J. Pirozzolo
derstanding and applying “gut feel” in decision-making. (Eds.), Multiple intelligences and leadership: 9 –28. Mah-
Academy of Management Executive, 18(4): 76 –91. wah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Saville, P., & Willson, E. 1991. Journal of Occupational Psychol- Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. 1995. Styles of thinking in the
ogy, 64: 219 –238. school. European Journal for High Ability, 6: 201–219.
Schutte, N., Malouff, J., Hall, E., Haggerty, D., Cooper, J., Golden, Torrance, E. P., McCarthy, B., & Kolesinski, M. T. 1988. Style of
D., & Dornheim, L. 1998. Development and validation of a learning and thinking. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing
measure of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individ- Service.
ual Differences, 25: 167–177.
Tsoukas, H. H. 1991. The missing link: A transformational view
Segal, E. 2004. Incubation in insight problem solving. Creativity of metaphors in organizational Science. Academy of Man-
Research Journal, 16(1): 141–148. agement Review, 16(3): 566 –585.
Seligman, M. E. P. 1998. Learned optimism: How to change your Walck, C. L. 1996. Management and leadership. In A. L. Hammer
mind and your life (2nd ed.). New York: Pocket Books. (Ed.), MBTI applications: A decade of research on the Myers-
Briggs Type Indicator: 55–75. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psy-
Senge, P. M. 1990. The fifth discipline: The art and practice of the
chologists Press, Inc.
learning organization. London: Random House.
Waldrop, M. M. 1992. Complexity: The emerging science at the
Shelton, C. D., & Darling, J. R. 2003. From theory to practice:
edge of order and chaos. New York: Simon and Schuster.
Using new science concepts to create learning organiza-
tions. The Learning Organization, 10(6): 353–360. Werhane, P. H. 1999. Moral imagination and management deci-
sion-making. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Siggelkow, N., & Rivkin, J. W. 2005. Speed and search: Designing
organizations for turbulence and complexity. Organization Wheatley, M. 1987. Leadership and the new science. San Fran-
Science, 16(2): 101–123. cisco: Barrett-Koehler.

Simon, H. A. 1987. Making management decisions: The role of Wong, C., Law, K., & Wong, P. 2004. Development and validation
intuition and emotion. Academy of Management Executive, of a forced choice emotional intelligence measure for Chi-
1(1): 57– 64. nese respondents in Hong Kong. Asia Pacific Journal of
Management, 21: 535–559.
Simon, H. A., Newell, A., & Shaw, J. C. 1979. The process of
creative thinking. In H. A. Simon (Ed.), Models of thought: Yaniv, I., & Meyer, D. E. 1987. Activation and metacognition of
195–214. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. inaccessible stored information: Potential bases for incu-
bation effects in problem solving. Journal of Experimental
Sipps, G. J., & DiCaudo, J. 1988. Convergent and discriminant Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13: 187–205.
validity of the Meyers-Briggs Type Indicator as a measure
Zaccaro, S. J. 2002. Organizational leadership and social intel-
of sociability and impulsivity. Educational and Psycholog-
ligence. In R. E. Riggio, S. E. Murphy, & F. J. Pirozzolo (Eds.),
ical Measurement, 48: 445– 451.
Multiple intelligences and leadership: 29 –54. Mahwah, NJ:
Skordoulis, R. T. 2004. Strategic flexibility and change: An aid to Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
strategic thinking or another managerial abstraction? Stra-
Zalewski, L. J., Sink, C. A., & Yachimowicz, D. J. 1992. Using
tegic Change, 13(5): 253–258.
cerebral dominance for education programs. The Journal of
Sloman, S. A. 1996. The empirical case for two systems of rea- General Psychology, 119: 45–57.
soning. Psychological Bulletin, 119: 3–22.
Zhang, L. 2002. Thinking styles similar to modes of thinking:
Smith, M. 1999. Personality issues and their impact on account- Thinking styles and modes of thinking: Implications for
ing and auditing. Managerial Auditing Journal, 14(9): 453– education and research. The Journal of Psychology, 136(3):
460. 245–261.

Charles M. Vance teaches graduates and undergraduates in the area of human resources at
Loyola Marymount University in Los Angeles. Much of his research and consulting has
focused on the design and management of university curriculum and corporate learning
systems in the United States and abroad. He recently completed dual Fulbright appointments
in China and Austria. Vance enjoys exercising his nonlinear creative side with his quarterly
editorial cartooning in his final “Out of Whack” section of the Journal of Management Inquiry.
He also occasionally sings and yodels for his students to potently illustrate a point in class.
2007 Vance, Groves, Paik, and Kindler 185

Kevin S. Groves (PhD, Claremont Graduate University) is assistant professor of management


and director of the PepsiCo Leadership Center at California State University, Los Angeles. His
research and consulting interests include managerial thinking styles, ethical decision mak-
ing, executive leadership development and succession planning systems, charismatic lead-
ership, and leader emotional intelligence. He teaches undergraduate, MBA, and doctoral-level
classes across a range of management, organizational behavior, and leadership subjects. Dr.
Groves’ recent research has been published in such journals as the Journal of Management,
Human Resource Development Quarterly, Leadership and Organization Development Journal,
and the Journal of Management Development.

Yongsun Paik (PhD, international business, University of Washington) is professor of interna-


tional business and management in the College of Business Administration, Loyola Mary-
mount University. His primary research interests focus on international human resource
management, global strategic alliances, and Asia Pacific business studies. Paik has recently
published articles in Journal of World Business, Management International Review, Journal of
International Management, Business Horizons, International Journal of Human Resource Man-
agement, Journal of Management Inquiry, and Human Resource Management Journal, among
others.

Herb Kindler (PhD), is emeritus professor at Loyola Marymount University and director of Herb
Kindler & Associates, a firm that conducts training programs on clear and creative thinking,
managing disagreement constructively, stress management, and leadership skill building.

S-ar putea să vă placă și